Reduction in secondary grammaticalization: Evidence from the Spanish periphrastic past Chad Howe, University of Georgia The correspondence between meaning and phonetic form has long been discussed in the literature on grammaticalization, which maintains that elements with more abstract (grammatical) meaning tend to be shorter and more reduced (Givón 1979, Bybee et al. 1994, Lehmann 1995). In their analysis of tonal erosion in Sinitic languages, Ansaldo and Lim observe that grammaticalized items “show phonetic erosion [reduction] when compared to their lexical counterparts” and that this erosion is in fact “[o]ne of the salient aspects of the grammaticalization process” (2002:345,360). Nevertheless, recent studies have questioned this claim, observing that phonetic reduction is not necessarily linked to more abstract (i.e. grammaticalized) meaning (see Amaral et al. 2013, Gradoville 2013). Following Traugott (2002), the current study assumes a distinction between 'primary' and 'secondary' grammaticalization, the latter referring to changes that do not involve a shift in grammatical category but rather extension of semantic/pragmatic meaning. By analyzing patterns of phonetic reduction of a form undergoing secondary grammaticalization, in this case the perfectivization of the periphrastic past (or perfect) in Peninsular Spanish (see Schwenter & Torres Cacoullos 2008), I argue that reduction is not a "salient" feature of secondary grammaticalization. Various studies, such as Bybee (2002), have shown that the rate of deletion (or reduction/lenition) of intervocalic /d/ in Spanish is greater with participles than with other lexical, non-grammaticalized classes (see also Blas Arroyo 2006). These analyses, based on binary methods of discerning phonetic attrition, largely ignore the autonomy of the phonetic processes in question, obfuscating what turn out to be patterns of erosion attested across all relevant phonetic contexts--e.g., increased lenition with -ado sequences (vs. -ido). The current analysis offers new evidence based on acoustic properties of Spanish intervocalic /d/, namely comparison of relative intensities between d and the following vowel, and demonstrates that sound change in morphosyntax is more gradient than observed by Bybee (2002, and others). The data, comprised of -ado and -ido sequences, were extracted from a corpus of spoken Spanish consisting of sociolinguistic interviews with speakers from Alcalá de Henares, Spain, a dialect in which the periphrastic past displays the perfect to perfective development characteristic of other Peninsular varieties (see Howe 2013). Using a mixed effects multivariate analysis in R (with speaker as a random factor), I analyzed the distribution of intensity ratios for each token, as demonstrated with the token he estado in Figure 1 below, where a value of 1 indicates no observable lenition (not attested in the data) and 0 no observable acoustic presence of the /d/ (adapted from Hualde et al. 2010). The quantitative analysis reveals no significant differences between the intensity ratios of the participle tokens and those of other -ado/-ido contexts (e.g., in nouns), as demonstrated in the box plot in Figure 2. The most operant factors in the statistical model are (i) the surrounding vocalic context, with -ido tokens displaying significantly lower rates of /d/ reduction and (ii) the frequency of the particular token, with high frequency elements showing greater /d/ reduction. These findings support the claim that forms undergoing secondary grammaticalization—i.e. those cases of change that specifically involve increased abstractness (e.g., the perfectivization of the Spanish perfect)—pattern similarly to other, non-grammaticalizing elements in the grammar in terms of their response to pressures of phonetic reduction. The results of the current study call into question Ansaldo and Lim's claim regarding the general role of phonetic reduction in linguistic change, suggesting instead that erosion in secondary change can be the result of factors that are not correlated per se with the process of grammaticalization. Figure 7: Normalized Intensity by Informant Figure 8: Normalized Intensity by Part of Speech Figure 1: Figure 2: Analysis Intensity Ratio = Peak(V2) - Valley(d) No significant effect for Part of Speech Peak(V2) The preferred method of analysis for this project is a regression model including interaction. Initially, eight different categorical variables will be used as predictors of the References: normalized dip values. These are Token, Region of Birth, Gender, Age, Task, TokenType, Amaral, Patrícia, Meghan Armstrong, & Luciana Lucente. 2013. Phonetic cues in the production of aspectual ,QIRUPDQW&RGHDQG:RUG7\SH:HZDQWHGWRILQGDUHOHYDQW³]HUR-KHDY\´GLVWULEXWLRQWKDW periphrases. Paper presented at LSRL 44. New York University. Ansaldo, Umberto and Lisa Lim. 2002. Phonetic absence as syntactic prominence: Grammaticalization in isolating total languages. and that down Cline-The Nature of and Grammaticalization, by O. would adjust for Up the fact ourthe data are heavily skewed that our errors willed. almost Fischer, M. Norde and H. Perridon, 345-362. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins. certainly be normally distributed. This, however, to be something we could not Blas Arroyo, José Luis.not 2006. Hasta aquí hemos llega(d)o: ¿Un turned caso deout variación morfológica? Factores estructurales y estilísticos en el español de una comunidad bilingüe. Southwest Journal of Linguistics 25.39-73. achieve, and we decided a linear regression would be sufficient for the data we have. The Bybee, Joan L. 2002.Word frequency and context of use in the lexical diffusion of phonetically variables includeLanguage in the finalVariation model were by the p-values of each individual conditioned sound to change. anddetermined Change 14.261–290. Givón, Talmy. 1979. On Understanding Grammar. New York: Academic Press. categorical variable and the two-way interaction variables. Elimination of a variable can occur if Gradoville, Michael. 2013. Grammaticization and phonetic reduction: The case of Caraqueño Spanish para. Paper presented at NWAV 42. University of Pittsburgh. it is deemed insignificant based on its associated p-value tabulated from the regression Howe, Chad. 2013. The Spanish Perfects. Basingstoke/New York: Palgrave Macmillan. Hualde, José Ignacio, Marianna & we Miquel Simonet. Lenition phonemic calculations. By thisNadeu, process, are left with the2010. variables Token,and Task, Region,contrast Gender, in and Majorcan Catalan. Romance Linguistics 2009: Selected papers from the 39th Linguistic Symposium on Romance (LSRL), Tucson, Arizona, March ed. by S. Colina, A. Olarrea and the Languages six relevant two-way interactions between these2009, variables. A.M. Carvalho, 63-80. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins. Lehmann, Christian. 1995 [1982]. Thoughts on grammaticalization. München / Newcastle: Lincom Europa. Schwenter, Scott A. & Rena Torres Cacoullos. 2008. Defaults and indeterminacy in temporal grammaticalization: The ‘perfect’ road to perfective. Language Variation and Change, 20.1–39. Traugott, Elizabeth C. 2002. From etymology to historical pragmatics. Studies in the History of the English Language, ed. by D. Minkova and R. Stockwell, 19-39. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
© Copyright 2024 ExpyDoc