Whole Language vs. Phonics Teaching Children to Read Presented by: Nicole Jackson October 25, 2005 Learning to Read • In a literate society learning to read is considered a necessary task. • The debating on this topic seems to come in waves. It was very heated in the late 60’s and it eased up for a while but was rekindled in the 90’s. • The debate has important contributions to the formation of school and government policies. – In 1997, the U.S. congress created a national panel to review and evaluate research on various approaches for teaching children to read The Contenders: Whole Language • A.k.a. Top down or meaning-emphasis approach to learning. • “natural” • Although the whole-language approach has grown from ideas in the language experience movement, the whole language approach as it is known today began in the late 70s with Kenneth and Yetta Goodman, who published the article “Learning to Read is Natural”. (Stahl 1994) • Children learn to recognize words by appearance and context. • The use of authentic/purposeful teaching moments is emphasized. The Contenders: Whole Language (cont’d) • • • • There is some confusion in discussing whole language because it has several meanings. Some view it as the integration of reading with all other forms of language learning such as speaking, writing, and listening. Others believe it is just an indirect approach to teaching the same things as any other approach. Others have expanded it to a philosophy of education and life! Whole language sees decoding as a byproduct that should be taught sparingly and incidentally. (Chall, 1996) Goal of developing attitude toward reading (Stahl et al., 1994) Recognized to contain a “number of features that can benefit children in many ways” (Simner, 1992). The Contenders: Whole Language (cont’d) • Belief in not pressuring children is often misunderstood as meaning the children should be left to their own devices. Evidence • Manning & Kamii, 2000 • Stahl, McKenna, & Pagnucco, 1994 The Contenders: Phonics • A.k.a. Bottom-up or code-emphasis approach to reading. • Children taught to associate sounds with individual letters and letter combinations. • Teaches strategies in “sounding-out” words • Phonics training includes more than just phonemic awareness, it also includes practice reading words in and out of text. Evidence • Eldredge, Quinn, & Butterfield, 1990 – All correlations between phonics, reading comprehension, and vocabulary were positive r(502)=.506 to .757, p < .001 • Bus & van Ijzendoorn, 1999 • Stahl, McKenna, & Pagnucco, 1994 • Foorman et al., 1991 Making Phonics Fun http://www.clicknkids.com/home.asp http://www.zoo-phonics.com/ http://www.jollylearning.co.uk/ http://www.sadlieroxford.com/phonics/control_page/fron t2.htm http://secure.hop.com/ Testing Issues • How is reading defined? – Pseudowords, comprehension, lists • Standardized reading achievement measures criticized for representing an “unnatural” reading act • Oral reading passages as a tool Different Strokes for Different Folks • Results for the teaching methods may vary between different groups of children. Specifically, children with learning disabilities or disadvantaged backgrounds may respond differently than a typical child . – Peoples & Nelson, 1977 – Bus & van IJzendoorn, 1999 • Differences between age groups tested – Bus & van IJzendoorn, 1999 – Stahl, McKenna, & Pagnucco, 1994 Getting Along • “Young children should be provided with many holistic reading experiences, and they should be encouraged to make sense out of everything they read” (Eldredge, Quinn, & Butterfield, 1990) • “The present results strengthen the case for a balanced perspective on reading instruction” (Bus & van IJzendoorn, 1999) • “Although whole-language advocates are opposed to piecemeal, work-sheet driven phonics instruction, most support phonics instruction that is embedded into the reading of authentic texts and into children’s discovery of spelling” (Stahl, McKenna, & Pagnucco, 1994) Graduate-level research Marvin Simner, PhD - University of Western Ontario Debra Jared, PhD - MSc. Degree - Cognition and Perception - University of Western Ontario Connie Varnhagen, PhD - MSc. Degree - Applied Developmental Science - University of Alberta References Bus, A.G., & Ijzendoorn, M. H. (1999). Phonological awareness and early reading: A meta-analysis of experimental training studies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(3), 403-414. Eldredge, J. L., Quinn, B., & Butterfield, D. D. (1990). Causal relationships between phonics, reading comprehension, and vocabulary achievement in the second grade. Journal of Educational Research, 83(4), 201-212. Foorman, B., Francis, D., Novy, D., & Liberman, D. (1991). How letter-sound instruction mediates progress in firstgrade reading and spelling. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83(4), 456-469. Manning, M., & Kamii, C. (2000). Whole language vs. isolated phonics instruction: A longitudinal study in kindergarten with reading and writing tasks. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 15(1), 53-65 Peoples, A. C., & Nelson, R. O. (1977). The differential effects of phonics versus sight-recognition on the eye movements of good and poor second-grade readers. Journal of Reading Behavior, 9(4), 327-337. References Simner, M. L. (1992). Section’s position paper on beginning reading instruction. Educational and School Psychology Newsletter. May 1992. Stahl, S., McKenna, M., & Pagnucco, J. (1994). The effects of whole-language instruction: an update and reappraisal. Educational Psychologist, 29(4), 175-185. Discussion • How did you learn to read? • Do you think further research is required in this area? • Can both methods be amalgamated into one all-accommodating teaching program?
© Copyright 2024 ExpyDoc