--;; , .,- ," Jfada International Journal of Business li. \0. 2 (May-August 2014): 93-109 iriiiiiii?6Nlr .l#LIRNAL'I st",g$xNKsg The Impact of Message Framing and Source Credibility on Breastfeeding Intention: A Social Marketing Approach# Ananda Sabil Hussein,^r Valeile Mannab " and David Cohenh Faculty of Economics & Business, Universias Brawljaya, Malang Indonesia b Faculty of Commerce Lincoln Univeniq'Canterbury, New Zealand q-ritract: Though highly recommended by health organizaaons woddwide, breastfeeding an infant from : ,--:. rcr a period of several months is not universal. There is thus a need to investigate appropriate and ,':;::r-e means to promote this breastfeeding behavior. This study, rooted in a scicial marketing perspecr :. :.sts the impact of message ftaming and source credibility on the behavioral intention to breastfeed. - - :; 2 factorial experiment was conducted in Indonesia, an especially relevant setting given that the :::,-ntage of Indonesian women who breastfeed is low compared to other countries. Two hundred and :' ::n- nine pregnant women participated in this study. The findings of this study indicate that the inter,:--,r between message framing and source credibility has a significant effect on a person's attitude and - =:jon to provide exclusive breastfeeding. In addition, this study finds that attitude is an essential deter- --.-::rr of intention. -1'bstrak: Meskipun sangat dianjurkan oleh organisasi-organisasi kesehatan secara global, meny.rsui bayi :,.:1ahir untuk iangkawaktu beberapa bulan tidak uniuersal. Oleh karena itu,adanya kebutuhan untuk - .:' :lidiki cara yan1 tepat dan efektif dalam rangka mempromosikan perilaku menyusui. Penelitian ini, -': rerdasar pada perspeknf social marketing dimana penelitian ini menguji dampakframingpesan dan :: : :i-iitas sumber pada niat berperilaku untuk menl'usui. Sebuah percobaan dengan menggunakan desain j,..: ial 2x2dllaksanakandilndonesia.Duaratustujuhpr,rluhsembilanperempuanhamilberpartisipasi -. =:: penelitian ini. Temuan dari penelitian ini menuniukkan bahvz interaksi antaraframing pesan dan :. i':Itas sumber memiliki pengaruh signifikan terhadap sikap dan niat untuk memberikan ASI eksklusif. r : - :- :iu, peneJitian ini menemukan bahwa sikap merupakan. ^Ke-ru'ords: attitude; framing; behavioral intention; breastfeeding; credibility # This is a part of Hussein's doctoral dissertation at Lincoln Universiq., New Zealand. ' Corresponding author's e-mail: [email protected] .:S\: 1141-1128 - :::: /u-rvw.g2maijb.mmugm.ac.id/ 93 HwseinetaJ. Introduction The way a message is framed can have a significant effect on the result of its communication (Rothpan and Salovey 1997). Message framing has been widely used in designing promotional messages, especially those seeking to influence individual behar'ior. I(ahneman andTversk/s (1979) Prospect Theory is one basis by which the impact of message framing on behar,'ior can be better understood. This theory explains that when presented in different ways, information can encourage an alteratton of people's perspectives, preferences and actions. This is reflected in practice, where message framing strategies have been used in manyhealth promotional messages involving both prevention and detection behaviors. To intluence people to adopt prevention behavior -a behavior designed to avoid the onse t of a health probIem- a positively-framed message is thought to be more effective than a negatively-framed message (R.othman and Salovey 1997). However, to influence individuals to engage in detection behavior, a negatively-framed me ssage is likely to be better than a positivelyframed message. Although this suggestion reflects the Prospect Theory's core proposition, empirical findings have been inconsistent in regards to these effects (Arora 2000; O'I(eefe and Jensen 2007; Rothman et al. 1993). The inconsistent findings suggest that there is value in testing the relationship in a different setting with different behaviors from those that have previously been examined. In addition, our incomplete knowledge on the impact ofmessage framing on behavior suggests that further study is needed to examine the credibility of the message source as an additional variable which could moderate the impact of message framing on an 94 individual's behavior. To date, the moderating effect of source credibility has been found in stud.ies on the promotion of goods or services (Arora et aL.2006; Jones et al. 2004; I{umkale et al. 201,0 ; Zhangand Buda 1,9 99). Though examples of studies focused on the role of source credibility in social marketing programs can be found, they arc relativell' rare. For example, I(im and I(im's (2014) study observed the effect of message framing and source credibility on individual's attitude s and intentions about green messages provided by a hotel. \Xtrile source credibiliq' has been found to moderate the effect of framing on behavioral intention and its antecedents, no studv has been found which addresses the potential interaction effect between message framing and source credibility with a paricular focus on breastfeeding behaviot Breastfeeding has been widely recognized as the perfect source of nutrition for babies for the first six months of their life. The term "exclusive breastfeeding" refers to the situation in which a baby is given only breast-milk with no supplementary food (a1though medicines and vitamins, if needed, conform to the definition). While it is esti- mated that an of 32 percent of ^verage women around the wodd who exclusir.el), breastfeed will do so for up to six months, the proportion of Indonesian mothers $/ho exciusive ly breastfeed is substantially less than that figure. It fluctuates from below 20 percent to less than 10 percent since 1991 (Statistics Indonesia and Macro International 2008). These very modest figures suggest that devising a strategy to increase the proportion ofwomen u'ho provide six months exclusive breastfeeding could make a valuabie contribution to Indonesian's public health. In addition, a better understanding of the impact of health message framing in gen- Gadiah Mada InternationalJournl of B,tsness - May,4tg.rt, t/01. 1 6, N0. 2, 201 4 erai could contribute to public health efforts )oth there and elsewhere. These gaps in our knowledge provide 'ustification for research that replicates and estends prior studies in orderto guna deeper :aderstanding of how to best stfucture per.uasive communications used for social marsering promotions tn alternative settings. Based on the recognizedresearch issues, study aims to detetmine the effect of ctedibility on the :.:titude and intention to provide six months -sclusive breastfeeding. In addition, this ':-idv also looks in detail at the relationship :-r\\:een attitude and intention. The results , i rhis study are expected to contribute to :'rth academic andpractical perspectives. For --:-e academic perspectivg, this study will pro-.-rde an understanding about the effect of ::ressage framingand source credibility on the .::itude and intention of people in the con:=rt of breastfeeding behavior. For the prac:cal perspective, this study will be the guide-res for social marketers and health practi:':ners to create a breastfeedingpromotional :.npaign. --::rs :nessage framing and source ing the desired behavior (I(eller and Thackeray 201,1). Alden et al. Q01.1) suggest that social marketing is different ftom ordinary health education. Parkinson et.al. Q011) argue that current breastfeedingpromotiors do not have any effect in motivating women to breas tfeed. In particular, they explain that people akezdy have knowledge about breastfeeding and its consequences. Hence, ordinary health education is not enough to influence individuals, to cafryr out this particular behavior. In order to promote new health behavior, social marketers rely on promotion (I(otler and Lee 2008). Specifically, promotion includes both the design and the delivery of persuasive communication in terms of inspiring the target market to take a particular action. Stubblefield (1,997) identifies two factors that seem to be particulady related to persuasive messages, namely the characteristics of the source and the characteristics of the message. Source factors telate to the person of orgxlzatton responsible fot the message andits distribution; meanwhile, message factors relate to the characteristics of the in- formation itself. Research Model and Hwotheses Social marketing has been recognized ,. an important approach to formulate strat-::'e s to change health behaviot (Secket et al. - ,'95). In terms of promoting new behavior, :::-rrketing communication is an important : -,mponent of social marketing (Alden et al. -.,11). In the general context of social marce ung promotion strategy, social communi:.dons plzy an important role in describing --:-- actions which the target audience should ::r. and in telling them the benefits of adopt- Sevetal studies have shown that framed messages have a significant effect on behav- ior change (Abhyankar et aL.2008; Banks et al. 1.995; Rivers c, ,1. 2005). With specific tegard to influencjn lrealth behavior, paying attentionto howa nlessage is framed has been shown to be effective for altering attitudes and subsequent behavior (Levin et al. 1998; Rothman and Salovey 1,997; Rothman et al. 2006).In recognition of this, the manipulation ofmessage framing has been widely utilized in sociai marketing efforts. The effect of message framing on peoples attitudes and intentions can be ex- 95 Husseinet al. plained by the Prospect Theory (Iversky and I{ahneman 1981). The Prospect Theory ar- havior with widely recognized benefits, it is classified in this study as low elaboration be- gues that people set an expected outcome as a reference point. Any outcome beyond this reference pointis considered a gain; and any- havior. thing less is viewed as a loss. The certainty of reahzingan outcome increases the degree to which people will seek to avoid a loss and increases how attractiye g rn is petceived ^ to be (I{ahneman and Tversky 1'979). Therefore, a persuasive rnessage being framed either in terms of gains or losses should be relevant to the preferences for a nnge of behaviors. With the present focus, where a specific behavior -breastfeeding- is the focus, the following hlpotheses can be generated: H,: Message in Hr: bgea sfee ding. framing has an impact 0n an indiuidual's intention to engage in breasfeeding. Researchers have also shown that the influential effect of a message on an individual's attitudes and intentions is moderated by the credibility of the message source (A.rora and Arora 2004; Jones et aI. 2003; Jones et zI. 2004).The current study uses the Elaboration Likelihood Model (ELX,[) proposed by Petty and Cacioppo (1986) as abasis for examining the moderating effect of source credibility on, first, the relationship between message framing and attitude, and second, between message framing and intention. The ELMexplains that, under low elaboratio n conditi ons, receivi ng information from a credible source would be likely to provide a cognitively easier route to accept the information given in a me ssage. In other words, if someone tfusts the source and easily understands the facts of the issue, there is little need to mofe thoroughly elabotate ofl, or debate, the contentions made in a message. Given that bteastfeeding is a natural be- 96 credibiliry Olson that the combi(1993) suggested andZanna nation of a positively-framed message with a highly credible source will be the most effective combination in promoting low elaborationbehavior Thereforg the credibility of the message soufce should moderate the persuasiveness of the message on afl individual's attitude and intention to provide exclusive breastfeeding. Thus we propose that: message framing and source Hr: There is a signtficant interaction message franing has an impact 0n an diuid aa I's attita de toward Message In terms of the interaction between ffict between Jraning and sowrce credibili\ on the attitade to prouide exclusiue breastfeeding. Given that breastfeeding is likely to be alow elaborationbehavior, as argued above, we expect that a positively-framed message with a high credibilitysource will be the most effective combination for affecting people's attitude. Hr: There is a signrficant interaction ruessage franing and intention to ffict betueen source credibiliry on the prouide exclusiue breasfeeding. Recognizing the Jinkage between attitudes and intentions, we expect that a positively-framed message with a high credibility source will be the most effective combination for affecting people's intentions. Several theories argue that attitudes exhibit strong influence on, and thus can be used to predict, intention. These perspectives include the Theory of Planned Behavior GPB) (Ajzen 1,991,), the Theory of Reasoned Action GRA) (Ajzen and Fishbein 1980) and theTriandis Model (friandis 1977). Ajzenand Fishbein (1980) enhance the potential of these theories by linking intention with behavior which is, ultimatell', a key outcome Sflla& &ure Intemationallournt of hrsines -Mayfugat, 1. The Reseatch W I 6 I{0, 2, Model marketing efforts. In the realm of ing behaviot, t\e hypothesized reb between attitudes and intention has onfirmed through empitical work et al. 1998; \ilfambach 1997). Thus that: /Wdc bas an ffict on a tuznan's intention lrcl*riue! breasfeed. These five hypotheses are graphically as Figure 1. Ate 201 4 sample of this study is pregnant The selection of the group of pregwas based on the argument statthe decision to start breastfeeding is rpde duringpregnancy (I(ramer et aI. llertens and Young 1,997). Furtherirrstudies have shown that intetven- tions such as peer suppoft andhealth education duringpregnancy will increase the likelihood of providing breastfeeding (Dyson et 21.2005; Ryan 1997). The study relied on a convenience sample drawn from a variety of locations. Participants wefe rectuited viz a face-to-face request made in several maternity hospitals, obstetrici an clinics, and brea stfe eding workshops located in Malang, EastJava Province, Indonesia. Three hundred and twenty questionnaires were distributed. Of these, 279 were completed, yielding a response rate of 87 percent. The following demographic profile emerged ftsn the sample: 68.1 percent of the to30 years, 60.1 percent have completed a tefrary degree, 54.8 percent are working mothers, and 54 percent have more than one child. Table 1 presents the respondent's demographics information respondents areagedbetween 21. Husseinet aJ,. Table 1. Demogtaphic of Sample Population Category Variable Younger than20 Ag. Education Job Number of Children (including in utero) t Measures of attitudes and behavioral intentions were generated from the basic contentions of the Theory of Planned Behavior (Ajzen1,991,; Duckett et al. 1998). The attitude toward exclusive breastfe eding wa s measured by six items using seven-point semantic differential scales. The scales utilized end points of weak - strong, unpersuasive - persuasive, unimportant - important, unbelievable - believable, uninformative - informative, not convincing - convincing, and bad afgument - good argument. Three seven-point Likert scales were employed to measufe the intention to exclusively breastfeed. 98 2.9 21 -30 68.1, 31, - 40 27.6 41 -50 1.1, High school graduate or less 36.9 Diploma 16.8 Bachelor's 40.1, Mastef's 3.2 Working 54.8 Not Working 43 1, 46 2 39.1, 3 12.5 4 0.7 Over Ques tionn afue Develop men o 5 1.5 Design and Procedures h 2 x 2 factonal design was used to measure the influence of message framing and source credibility on a woman's intention to breastfeed. Since the design consists of four factors, each discrete possible value and all expedmental units take onall possible combinations of these levels across all such factors; the factorial design ptepared in this study is considered as a full factortal experiment. Four different adverti sing pamphlets were designed to provide the unique combinations of positive and negative fnming as well as low and high source credibility. Ma- I h Mada IntemationalJoutrnl of Brniner;s - Itfuy'4ryut, Vul I 6 Na nipulation of the positively-framed message uas done by presenting arguments that outfned the gains/benefits to be derived from breastfeeding. In contrast, the negatively 2, 2U 4 ftamed m€ssage identified the benefits lost by not engagingin the behavior Table 2 summarizes the framing manipulation used in this study. Trble 2. Messhge Ftaming Manipulation Itoeitively Ftamed Me ss age -rhat supplying exclusive breastfeeding 5r &e first six months -with continued hasfeeding fo.tthe first year, ptovides sev- cr{ benefits for both baby and mother. Negatively Framed Message ...reported that not providing exclusive breastfeeding for the first six months with continued breastfeeding for the first year could potentially te sult in serzet al dis advantages for both baby and mother. hbaby: For baby: - fttrntially save 1.3 million lives - Gmtaining all the vitamins and minerals roded by a young baby, - Lc'easing the level of antibodies - Potentially lead to 7.3 million deaths every ye f - Less chance to receive complete nutrition, rihcing the chance of infection - Hd a health digestion system - Less eftctive antibodies - Incteasing the chance of getting infec- - vitamins and minerals tion - Weakening m&er: *tcing pregnancies Lecingthe chance to get ovananenGT the digestive system For mother: - Less chance to space pregnancies - Having more potential to get ovaiancancer Dccreasing the risk of breast cancer lDrrrcrnigally cheaper than botde feed- I - Mote likely to get breast cancer - Economically more expensive than breastfeeding Lrtry will have more chance of being and suongand the mother will have chance of avoiding many health probexclusive breas tfee ding ptovides hcalth benefits compared with probotrle-feeding or other solid and liq- ... ababy will have less chance of being hedthy and strongand the mother will have less ability to avoid many health problems. Providing botde-feeding or other solid and liquid food potentially will not provide as many benefits as six months exclusive breastfeeding Husseinet aL Table 3. Soutce Credibitty Manipulation High Credibility Source Low Credibility Soutce B]': Dr Ana Sofi,vanti, M.I{es., Ph.D., SPA (I{ons) By: Ana Sofiyanti Undergaduate Student Faculty of EconomicsJaya Manggala Universiry Jakafta Regarding the message credibility r.'anable, the high credibility message was presented by a (fictitious) breastfeeding exPert and also included a logo designed to represent a breastfe edingbased organization, v'hile the 1ow credibility message was signed by a university student and did not appear with any organi:zati on's logo. The tesearch stimulus was in the form of a booklet, which contained instructions and information relating to the study, message stimuli . and a questionnaire. To begin the experiment, an information sheet was distributed when the potential participants ardved at the registtation desk. After reading the information sheet and agreeing to participate, the participants were given brief instructions, asking them to read the article and compiete the attached questionnaire. The participants were given one of the four randornly selected stimulus messages. Reading the article and completing the questionnaire took participants from 15 to 20 minutes. During the experimental session, participantswere allowed to stop at any time. In addition, participants were allowed to ask the experimenter for assistance if they had anv difficulties in reading and understanding either the message stimulus or the questionnaire. 100 Data Analysis Parttal Least Square regression (PLS) was use d to analyze the data and test the hypotheses. Specificaily, Smart PLS with a bootstrapping re-sampling procedure lvas employe d. PLS was used since this technique can handle both formative and reflective indicators. In this studl', ftaming and credibility are the formative indicators, whiie attitude and intention are the reflective indicators. Furthermore, the ability of PLS to ana lyze data which have non-notmal distributions is anothet advantage. Finally, PLS has the abilit1, to contfol measufement ertor and can enhance testing of nomological webs among muitiple dependent variables. PLS thus is able to contribute to an improved experimental tesearch analysis in social sciences (Streukens et al. 2010). Forthis study, to model the experimental data with the framework of a structurai equation, single indicators for the manipuiated factors, namely, message framing and source credibilitv as well as the interaction effect are specified. These manipulations utihze effect coding, such as a positively-framed message is coded as 1, and a negativeh-framed message as 0, and a highly credible source is coded as 1, and a lovr credible source Gadiah Mada Internationauournal of Btrsiness -MayArqut,I'al. / 6,li,,. 2, :s 0. Since the manipulations do not have the :, pical propertie s of a reflective scale and it :s assumed that the manipulation itself is :re asured without error, this study depicts the :rl :rnipul a tr ons a s formative indi ca tors. \X'hile ---re manipulations afe fepresented as forma-r'e indicators, the dependent variables - at:r.ide and intention afe fepresented as reflec:r-e indicators. Manipulation Checks Two tests examined whether the stimuli :id any impact onperceptions. The first mea. -red the manipulation of credibilit,v. Parici:::rts wefe asked to fate the two versions of --:r- message on six seven-point semantic dif:::ential scales, with the end points being -: ustrvorthl' *not trustrirorthy, open minded -:r,lr open minded, good -bad, expeft -not :!)ert, experienced -not expefienced, and -:.tred -not trained. The mean difference be- I::le 20/ 4 tween the high credibility source (HC) and low credibilitl, sent.. (LC), where meanHc= 6.29 and meanlcr 4.13, was significant (p< .001), indicating that the intended difference in the message soufce's credrbility level was perceived by the respondents. A seven-point semantic differential was also use d to measure the framingstimuli. This use d end-points of positive to rate -negative perceptions on how the message was framed. The comparison of the mean scofes of the positively-framed message (meanor.= 6. 78) and negatively-framed message (mean^,.= 3.78), yielded a significant difference (p< .001). This demonstrared that the rwo frames were perceived as different bv the sample. Results Before testing the hl,potheses, it begins with teliability, convergent validity and discriminant validity checks. Table 4 provides 4. Construct Loadings Code Factof Loading Atd_1 0.751 Atd-2 0.81 7 Atd_3 0.80 Atd*4 0.762 Atd_5 0.799 Atd_6 0.75 -{.rritude --- rple a sant f Ple asant I rbarrassing/Not embarrassing '-:,healthy /Healthy 1-lu1sive/Appealing /Con venie n t -: --on r-e nie n - rr-atural/Natural t 1 B lntention - :rpect... Lrt_1 0.751 - ;--rnt. .. Int_2 0.78 9 - -:lrend... Int 0.729 . 3 ..to provide my baby with six months of exclusive breastfeeding 101 Husseinet al. With the outer model assessment indicating robust measufes, we then tested the hypotheses. Findings indicate that message framing does not have a significant main effect on eitherattitude (t= 1.534, p>0.05), ot intention (t= 1.424, p> 0.05). This leads to the rejection of both Hypotheses 1 and 2. the PLS parameter estimates for the measurement model. The factorloadings for all scales are ^cceptable, indicating robust measures. Table 5 explains the measures for examininginternal consistency of a given block of indicators based on composite reliability (Hair et aL.2010) and Average Variance Extracted (AVE) (Fornell and Larcker 1981). The composite reliability for the constructs tested exceed the cut-off value of 0.70 proposed by Nunnally.(l978). The AVE for the constructs also exceeds the cut-off value of 0. In terms of moderating effects, it was found that the source ctedibility significandy moderates the effect of message framing on attitude (t= 12.602, p< 0.01). This result supports Hypothesis 3. Since there is a significant interaction effect between message framing and source credibility on attitude, Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) was used to investigate the nature of this interaction. Analysis indicates that the Estimated Marginal Mean (EMXO of a positively-framed message coupled with a high source credibility is the highest @MNt= 5.756) comparec to other framing and source credibility cornbinations (positively-framed message witi low source credibility EMM= 5.236, negz tively-framed message with high credibilir' source EMM= 5.155 and negatively-ftamemessage with low credibility source EMII= 50 suggested by Fcrnell and Larcker (1 981 ). Following Chin's (1 998) guidelines, two methods were used to check discriminant validity. The first was to compare the factor loadings and cross loadings. To avoid having a discriminant validity problem, the factor loading coefficients should be higher than their respective cross loadings. Table 5 indicates that the value of factorloadings foreach construct is higher than the value of its respective cross loading. The second method was to compare the value of square root of AVE with the inter-correlation between constructs. Table 5 shows that the value of square root of AVE is higher than the intercorelation among the constructs. Based on these outer model evaluations, it can be said that the measures used in this study are robust. 5.671). Thus, in terms of influencin: women's attitudes to provide six months -: exclusive breastfee ding, a positively-fran:= : message coming from a highly credible sou:r: is the most effective combination. Horver--: Table 5. Composite Reliability, AVE and Correlation among Consttucts A\rE Reliability 102 {^,ru Composite Intention 0.573 0.80 Attitude 0.611 0.904 1 Intention Attitude 0.756 0.696 0.781 i &4h Mada IntemationalJouml of Bueiness -Iftayfuwt, Vul 6 M. 1 2, 2A4 rt is noteworthy that the combination of the negaliysly-fiamed message with the low cre& ilility sorrce yielded an EEM impact on atrimde thatwas not substantially different from positive-framed message, high credibility lrxlrce EEM (5.756 versus 5.671,). -' fgure 2. The significant moderating effect of sorrce credibiJity was also found in the relatibnship between message framing and intention (t= 3,079; p< 0.01), supporringHypothesis 4. ANOVA was again used to determine the nature of the interaetion, with outcomes lnteraction between Framing and Source Credibility on Attitude and Intention Estimatexd Marginal Means of Attitude c -Low -Iow crdility crdility c6 o a d .s o0 z .o x q, d E .E lrl Negatively-firamed Positively-fnmed Estimatexd Marginal Means of Intention c -Lwaodillr -I.ow d o a d c '5n ! a X o d E 'd rI] Neptivdyftrned Positively-frrmed edility I Husseinet al. indicating that a positively-framed message coming from a relativelv high credibility source was the most effective combination in affectingintention. The r.aiue of the EMM for this combina,tion (5.910) is the largest among the other combinations (positivelyfiamed message and low credibilitv source EMM= 5.497, negativell-framed message andhigh- credibiJity source EMNI= 5.333 and negatively-framed and lou' credibility source EN{NI= 5.835). Aswith the framingquestion nation (R), message ftarningand the interaction of message framing and credibiiitv explarns roughly a quafter Q4.6Vo) of the variance in attitude. In addition, 50.8 percent of the variance in intention was expiained b1' attitude, messa€le framing and the interaction of message framing and source credibility. Table 6 summaizes the results of the hypoth- results desc ribed above, the contf a st betrve en eses testing. the impacts of the positive-frame with high credibility and negative-frame and iow credibiliw onintention were of comparable magnitudes (5.910 versus 5.835), F[ure 2 depicts the interaction effect of message framing and source credibilitl' on attitude and intention. Hlpothesis 5's proporiiion that there is a significant effect of attitude on intention v/as supported by the data (t=I2.782; p< 0.01 , B = 0. 6 1 5). Attitude was shown to positivelv influence intention; the more positive the attitude of rvomen towatd providing six months of exclusive breastfeedirig the higher their intention to exclusively breastfeed. Referring to the coefficient of determi- To ensure the robustness of the research model, this study also uses the Goodness of Fit (GoF) index. Since PLS is a variancebased SEN{, it does not harre a formal GoF Therefbre, the calculation of GoF refers to Tenenhaus et al's (2004) srygestion. Derived from Tenenhaus et aI. (2004), Daryanle s1 al. (2010) proposed that the baselines forGoF are small= 0.1, medium= 0.25 and large= 0.3(r.The GoF for this study (0.471) is quite robust. Table 6. Hypotheses Testing Summary Hypothesis 104 Path t-value H1 Message Framing -+ Attitude 1..534 NS H2 Message Framing -) 1.424 NS H3 H4 MF * SC -> Attitude MF * SC -+ Intention H5 Attitude -+ Intention Intention 1.2.602 Supported 3.07 9 Supported 12.782 Supported 'r:irh Mada IntemationalJoumal of Busines - Jlay,4tgli,lhl. l6,l,o.2,2014 Discussion The hlpothesized direct relationshrps framing and attitude, and and intention were not nessage framing - .:rd. These results are inconsistent with r-,se obtained in studies by Arora (2000, : ,-) andJones et aL. (2004). The first pos.-:-e expianation for this difference is cen:.:ed on the nature of breastfeeding behavr.- :\\-een 1) message :. \Iany campaigns have been launched to :: ,rnote exclusive breastfeeding behavior in -:::onesia; for example, in 2010 UNICEF . -:norted a mobile media-based advertising :,:rpaign to raise awafeness of the benefits - e rclusive breastfeeding for children. In ,::r:ion, and with respect to demographic :--.r:cteristics, around 54 percent ofthe cur-=:-, study's participants haVe more than one : --d. suggesting that breastfeeding and its -:-.:ed issues afe not new to thern. Thus, it is :.. -.ible that this study's participants were ,-:;;drof the benefits of exclusive ^ware ::=.stfeeding and, by recognizingthose ben.::'. already had a positive attitude toward :::-;sive breastfeeding. These are attitudes --cn could have been formed through prior ',, :::, 'sure to similar messages, personal expe-:::ce or, perhaps, as a result ofgeneral soci::=- :uppoft of the practice. Therefore, there . possibility that any stimulus provided i . '-ci not have had anincremental effect on :':--.-ious favorable evaluations of the prac- :::The belief that they and their children .:= certain to tezltze the benefits of :;.s rteeding is another possible explanation i ::re insignificant main effect of message :.:.::::nq on women's attitudes towards pro' :-:: exclusive breastfeeding. If the argu-::-:s presented by the advertisement con-' ::: ro aheady held attitudes, it is feasonr -.: ro expect Little or no impact of framing - -" on the attitude. The Prospect Theory (I(ahneman and Tversky 197 9) posits that for relatively lovz risk behaviors, gain-framed or positivell.framed messages will be more persuasive than loss-or negatively-framed messagrs. Thus, iow risk behaviors can be thought of as having little risk of not realizing the s, do this and these expected outcomes -that outcomes will occur" Altematively, although breast milk has been clinically demonstrated to be the complete nutrition fot infants (Galson 2008), it is possible that message framing alone was not enough to overcome the thought that the outcomes of adopting this behavior will vary between infants and mothers. In addition, because positive outcomes are not easy to discern, perhaps thete is an underlying concern that they will not be attained. Yet, when the positively-framed message is deliveted by a credible source, there are differences found in both the attitudes towards exclusive breastfeeding and the intention to engage in exclusive breastfeeding. Perhaps while framing alone cannot dispel perceptions of risk, the addition of a credible message source may lower to a degree the possible uncertainfy that positive outcomes will be achieved and, consistent with the tenets of ELM, make the message easier to internalize. Ftaming alone does not establish the type of connection with the target audience that framing in conjunction with a credible message source appears to provide. If the focus is shifted from the effect of external stimuli, such as an advertisement that could be use d in a social marketing campaign, and looks instead at the psychologv behind the motivation, or intention, to engage in a certain behavior, the Theory ofPlanned Behavior (TPB) can be used as a basis to understand this study's findings. Based on the TPB 105 I Husseinet al. (Ajzen L99I), an individual's intention to adopt a desired behavior was affected by three robust variables; attitudes, perceived behavioral control (PBC) and subjective norms. In accorda4ce to the TPB approach and several studies in the area of breastfeeding behavior(Dodgson et al. 2003; Duckett et al. L998; Wambach 1997), this study found that the women's favorable attitude toward providing exclusive breastfeeding has a significant positive effect on their intention to exclusively breastfeed. This atti- tude is unlikely to be something formed by one exposure to an advertisement, but nther it is formed over time and with repeated exposure to the stimulus, or as a result of different sources of influence pointing to the same basic evaluation. Advertising can certainly serve as one of those sodrces tn alarger social marketing campaign. Based on the research findings, the most significant theoretical contribution of this research is the extension of the body of knowledge by integrating ELM and the Prospect Theory in investigating the effect of message framing and source credibility on behavioral intention and attitude in the area of social marketing. Specifically, the present study provides a comprehensive evaluation of the effect of message framing and the moderating effect of source credibility on an individual's attitude and intention to provide exclusive breastfeeding behavior This study validates previous studies such as Abhyankar et al. (200 8), Jones et aI. (200 4) and O'Connor et al. (2005), who found that message frzming does not have a significant main effect on behavioral intention and attitude. Meanwhile, this study challenges the findings of Jones et al. Q004) who investigated the effect of message framing and source credibility on behavioral interition and its predictors. \X4rile Jones' study did not find significant 106 interaction effect berween message framing and source credibilityon behavioral intention and other socio-cognitive variables, this research found a significant interaction effect where a positively-framed message coming from a high credibility source was the most effective combination in influencing behavioral intention and attitude. The combination of a positively-framed message which comes from a highly credible source supports the prediction of the Prospect Theorl' (I(ahneman and Tversky 1979), which suggested that an individual will avoid risk in the domain of gains, and the ELM (Petty and Cacioppo 19 86) which claimed that under the low elaboration condition, a high credibility source will be more persuasive than a low credibility source. Limitations and Recommendations The primary limitation of this study is that it does not measufe the actual behavior of interest; instead, it only measures the intention of women to exclusively breastfeed. This limitation is common to many studies utilizing intention as a dependant construct. It is recommended that a future study could move beyond measurement ofthis behavioral proxy and could include a measure of actual behavior. Another limitation is in regard to the sample rectuitment. tJilhile this study obtained a relanvely large sample (n = 279), respondents were pfegnant mothers recruited during a brief time span from several hospitals, matemity dinics and breastfeeding workshops in Malang, Indonesia. Hence the sampie does not represent the population of pregnant Indonesians and the findings thus marnot be genera[zed to either non-pregnanr mothers or pregnant women in other places. Finally, since this study only investigated the Gr&h fixda fnunationalJoutnal of hrsi'nas - May,&gast, Vll. I 6 M. 2, 2014 drect relationships among constfucts, any frture study could also investigate indirect knowledge about persuasive communication, message framing, source credibility and be- rdationships to provide more comprehensive havioral change. Refetences ft*r.g "tbhpnkar, P, D. B. O'Connor, and R. Lawton 2008. The role of message vacciration: Evidence of in promoti4g a loss-frame advantage. Pslcbokgy, Health and Mediciru 13 (1): 1. Apeq I.1,991.. The theory of planned behavior. OrganiqationalBebauir andHumanDecision 21,7. Process MMR 50:1,79- ! .!flren, I., and M. Fistrbein 7980. UnderstandingAttitudrs and Prediding Social Behauior. Prentice-Hall. Jl5co, D. L., M. D. Basit and S. Destrpande. 2071. Communications in social marketing. In G. Hastings, K. Angus, and C. A. Bryant @dt), The SAGE Handbuk of Social Marketing. London: SAGE Publications Ltd. Am,ra" R. 2000. Message framng and credibilityr application in dentd services Healtb Marketingpuarteill 18 (1-2):29. fmr4 R. 2007. Message frami4g strat%ies for new and mature products lomul of Produa and Brand Maragement 16 (6): 377-385. drcre, R., and A. Arora. 2004.T1:r.. irnpact of message frami4g and credibility. SeruireMarketirgpaarterll 26 (1):35 -53. "erora, R, C. Stoner, and A. Arora . 2006. Using framing and ctedibility to irrcoqporate exercise and fitness in individuals' hfestty'e. Journal of Consumr Marketing % @: 199 - 207 . fdqSM.,P. of Salovey,S. Greener,A,J. Rothman,A. Moyer,J.Beauvais,andE. Epel. 1995.The effects message Dqrafto, n., K. regulatory franirg on mammography utilization. Heahb Pgclnhgy 1,4: 1,78-778. de Ruyter, and M. $Tetzels 2010. Getti4g a discount or sharing the cost The influence of fit on corsumer response to service pricrg schemes lournal of Seryite Rsurch 13 Q): 153-167. brfusorlJ. E., S.J. Henly, L. Duckett, and M. Thrrant. 2003.Theory of planned behavior-based models for bteastfeeding duration among Hong Kong mothers NarsingRcrearcb 52 p): M& Eett, L., S. Henly, M. Avery S. Pottet, S. Hills-Bonczyk, R.Huldeq and I{. Savik. 1998. planned behavior-based strucn:rd model for breast-feedtng. NursingResearcb 47 A theory of (6). &mq f L., E McCormick, and M, J. Renfrew. 2005. Interventions for promoting the initiation of breasdeeding. Cuhrane Database Slst Rn 2. E#l, C., and D. E Latcker. 1981. Evaluati4g strucural equation models with unobservable vadables and measuremerit effor. lournal of Marketing resarch 18 (1): 39-50. S K. E A). 2008. Mothers and childrenbenefit from breastfeedi4g. Jonrnalof tbeAnericanDietetic Assuiation 108 (7): 7106 - 1707. ElE,nfiC.Black,B.J.BabiaandD.R.Andetson 2010.MultimriateDataArafusirAGlo;batPerEatiue Cf* .d). Upper Saddle River: Pearson Education Inc. t07 HussehetaJ,. Jones, L. ![, R. C. Sinclair, and K. S. Courne va. 2003.The effects of source credibility and message ftaming on exercise intentions, behaviors and attitudes: An integration of the elaboration likelihood model and prospect theory. Journal of Applied Socia/ Pychologt, 3J(1),1,79 - 1,96. Jones, L. \V, Sinclak, R. C., Rhodes, R. E., and Courneya, K. S. (2004). Promoting exercise behaviour: an integration of "persuasion theories and the theory of planned behaviour. Bitish jotu'nal of health py ch ology, 9 (4), 505-521,. I{ahneman, D., and A. Tversky. 1979. Prospect theory: An analysis Journal of the Econometric S ocie!: 263-291' . of decision under risk. Econometrica: I(eller, H., and R. Thackeray. 201 1. Social matketing and the cteative process Heahh Promotion Practice 1.2 (5): 651-653. Kim, S.-8., and D-Y.'I<m.2014. The effecm of message framing and soutce credibility on green messages in hotels Cornell Hospitali4, puaterfi 55 (1): 64-75. P., and N. R. Lee. 2008. SocialMarketing InfkncingBehaborsfor Good (3'd ed.). Thousan Oaks: Sage PubLications Kotler, I{ramer, M. S., B. Chalmers, E. D. Hodnett, Z. Sevkovskaya, I. Dzikovic}r, S. Shapiro, J. P Colleg I. Vanlovictql.MezeryandT. DucrueLnC1. Promotionof bteastfeedurg inerventbn trial (PROBIT). J.4MA: thejournal of the American Medical Association 285 (4): 413-420. I(umkale,G. T.,D. Albarraciqand P.J. Seignourel.2010. Theeffecs of source credibiliryinthepresence or absence of prior attitudes: Implications for the design of petsuasive communication campatgns Journal of Appbed S ocial Pslchology a0 (6): 1325-1,356. Leviq I. P, S. L. Schneider, and G.J. Gaettr- 1998. All frames arerrot created equal A gpology and cdtical analysis of framing effects Organrqatnnal Behauior and Human Decision ProcessesT6 (2):149-188. I{. Young. 1997. Determinants of breastfeeding in four CanadtanOJibwa commuA decision making model. Ameican Journalof Human Bnlogy 9 (5): 579-593. Martens, P.J., and T. nities: Nunnally, l. 1 97 8. P1 v ho me tri c T h e or1. New York: M cGrav' -HilL O'Connor, D. B., E. Ferguson, and R. C. O'Connor. 2005.Intentions to use hormonal male contraception: The role of mess€e framing, attitudes and stress appraisals Biti:h Journalof Pychology 96 (3): 351.-369. J., andJ. D. Jensen. 2007.The relative persuasiveness of gain-framed loss-framed messages encouraging disease prevention behaviors: Ameta-analytic review. Joarnal of Health Corumunica- O'I{eefe, D. for tion 1.2 (7): 623 - 644. Olson,J. M.,and M.P.Zaruw.1993. Attitudesand attitudechange. AnnualRwiew of Pgtchokgy 44(1):117- {t 1,54. Parkirsoq #, ,-; J., R. Russell-Bennett, and J. Previte . 201,1,. aky. Aus tra lasi an ': Mark e ting J o wr na Mum or bub? \X4rich influences breastfeeding loy- I (AMJ). Petty, R. E., and J. T. Cacioppo. 1986. The elaboration likelihood model mmtal socialpgchologl 19 (1): 123-205. of persuasion Aduanas in experi- Rivers, S. E,., P Salovey', D. A. Pizarrq J.P'aano, and T. R. Schneider. 2005. Message framing and pap test utilization among women attending a community heahh clinic. /a wrnal af Health Pychobgy 10 (1): 65. Rothman, A. J., and P Salovey. 1997. Shaping perception message frami4g. Pslchobgical Bulleting 1.21 (1): 3 - 1,9. 108 to motivate heahhy behavior: The role of Ga diah Mada Intemationa{ournal of Busitess - May.Aryl t, W 1. 1 6, Nr. 2, 201 4 Rothman, A.J., R.D. Bartels,J. nflJaschiq and P. Salovey.2006. The strategrc use of gain-and loss-framed messages to promote healthy behavior: How theory cafl inform practice. Journal of Communication 56 (s1):202-220. I{. I(eougir, and C. D. Martin. 1993. The id-luence oi message framing on intentions to perform health behaviors. Journal af Expeinntal SocialPg,.ho/agt 29 (.5): Rothman, A.J., P. Salovey, C. Antone, 408-433. doi: R1zn, A. 1 0.1 006/ jesp. 1 993.1 S. 1997. The resurgence 01 9 of breastfeeding in the United States. Pediatrics 99 @: e1.2-e12. Secker, J., E. \7imbush,J. Watson, and I{. Milburn. 1995. Qualitative methods in searclu some criteria for qualiry. Health E dacation lournal54 (1):74. heakhpromotion re- Statistics Indonesia, and Macro International. 2008. Indonuia Denographic andHealth Suruq 2007.MaryJand: BPS and Macro International Streukens, S., M. \feuels, A. Daryanto, and I{. De Ruyter. 20i0. AnalyAng factonal experimental data using PI-S: An altemative approachand applbationin anonline complaining conto(t. InV EspositoVinzi W Chin, J. Henseler and H. Wand (Eds.) , Handbook of Panial l-.east Squares: Conceptq Methods a n d App li c ati ors Springer. Stubblefield, C.l,99l.Persuasivecommunication:Marketingheaithpromotion.l{ursingOutlook45:1.73171. Tenenhaus, M., S. Amato, anfl V Esposito Yirut2004. A global goodness-of-fitindex for PLS sffuctwal equation modeiling. Paper presented at the Proceedings of the XLII SIS scienti{ic meetrng. Triandis, H. C.1,977. Interpersonalbehailon Brooks/Cole Pub. Co. Tr-erslq',A.,andD. I{ahneman. l98l.Theframingof decisionsandthepsychologyof choice. Science,211. (4481):453. N-ambaclr, K. A. 1997. Breasdeeding intention and outcome: Raearch in Nursing and Health 20 (I):51-60. A test of the theory of planned behavior. Zhang, Y, and R. Buda. 1999. Moderating fffects of need for cognition on responses to positively versus negatively framed advertising messages Jowrnalof Aduertiing28 (2): I-15. 109
© Copyright 2024 ExpyDoc