The Impact of Message Framing and A Social Marketing Approach#

--;;
,
.,-
," Jfada International Journal of Business
li. \0. 2 (May-August 2014): 93-109
iriiiiiii?6Nlr
.l#LIRNAL'I
st",g$xNKsg
The Impact of Message Framing and
Source Credibility on Breastfeeding Intention:
A Social Marketing Approach#
Ananda Sabil Hussein,^r Valeile Mannab
"
and
David Cohenh
Faculty of Economics & Business, Universias Brawljaya, Malang Indonesia
b
Faculty of Commerce Lincoln Univeniq'Canterbury, New Zealand
q-ritract: Though highly recommended by health organizaaons woddwide, breastfeeding an infant from
: ,--:. rcr a period of several months is not universal. There is thus a need to investigate appropriate and
,':;::r-e means to promote this breastfeeding behavior. This study, rooted in a scicial marketing perspecr :. :.sts the impact of message ftaming and source credibility on the behavioral intention to breastfeed.
- - :; 2 factorial experiment was conducted in Indonesia, an especially relevant setting given that the
:::,-ntage of Indonesian women who breastfeed is low compared to other countries. Two hundred and
:' ::n- nine pregnant women participated in this study. The findings of this study indicate that the inter,:--,r between message framing and source credibility has a significant effect on a person's attitude and
- =:jon
to provide exclusive breastfeeding. In addition, this study finds that attitude is an essential deter-
--.-::rr of intention.
-1'bstrak: Meskipun sangat dianjurkan oleh organisasi-organisasi kesehatan secara global, meny.rsui bayi
:,.:1ahir untuk iangkawaktu beberapa bulan tidak uniuersal. Oleh karena itu,adanya kebutuhan untuk
- .:' :lidiki cara yan1 tepat dan efektif dalam rangka mempromosikan perilaku menyusui. Penelitian ini,
-': rerdasar pada perspeknf social marketing dimana penelitian ini menguji dampakframingpesan dan
:: : :i-iitas sumber pada niat berperilaku untuk menl'usui. Sebuah percobaan dengan menggunakan desain
j,..: ial
2x2dllaksanakandilndonesia.Duaratustujuhpr,rluhsembilanperempuanhamilberpartisipasi
-. =:: penelitian ini. Temuan dari penelitian ini menuniukkan bahvz interaksi antaraframing pesan dan
:. i':Itas sumber memiliki pengaruh signifikan terhadap sikap dan niat untuk memberikan ASI eksklusif.
r : - :- :iu, peneJitian ini menemukan bahwa sikap merupakan.
^Ke-ru'ords: attitude; framing; behavioral intention; breastfeeding; credibility
# This is a part of Hussein's doctoral dissertation at Lincoln Universiq., New Zealand.
' Corresponding author's e-mail: [email protected]
.:S\: 1141-1128
- :::: /u-rvw.g2maijb.mmugm.ac.id/
93
HwseinetaJ.
Introduction
The way a message is framed can have
a significant effect on the result of its communication (Rothpan and Salovey 1997).
Message framing has been widely used in designing promotional messages, especially
those seeking to influence individual behar'ior. I(ahneman andTversk/s (1979) Prospect
Theory is one basis by which the impact of
message framing on behar,'ior can be better
understood. This theory explains that when
presented in different ways, information can
encourage an alteratton of people's perspectives, preferences and actions. This is reflected in practice, where message framing
strategies have been used in manyhealth promotional messages involving both prevention
and detection behaviors. To intluence people
to adopt prevention behavior -a behavior
designed to avoid the onse t of a health probIem- a positively-framed message is thought
to be more effective than a negatively-framed
message (R.othman and Salovey 1997). However, to influence individuals to engage in
detection behavior, a negatively-framed me ssage is likely to be better than a positivelyframed message. Although this suggestion
reflects the Prospect Theory's core proposition, empirical findings have been inconsistent in regards to these effects (Arora 2000;
O'I(eefe and Jensen 2007; Rothman et al.
1993). The inconsistent findings suggest that
there is value in testing the relationship in a
different setting with different behaviors
from those that have previously been examined.
In addition, our incomplete knowledge
on the impact ofmessage framing on behavior suggests that further study is needed to
examine the credibility of the message source
as an additional variable which could moderate the impact of message framing on an
94
individual's behavior. To date, the moderating effect of source credibility has been found
in stud.ies on the promotion of goods or services (Arora et aL.2006; Jones et al. 2004;
I{umkale et al. 201,0 ; Zhangand Buda 1,9 99).
Though examples of studies focused on the
role of source credibility in social marketing
programs can be found, they arc relativell'
rare. For example, I(im and I(im's (2014)
study observed the effect of message framing and source credibility on individual's attitude s and intentions about green messages
provided by a hotel. \Xtrile source credibiliq'
has been found to moderate the effect of
framing on behavioral intention and its antecedents, no studv has been found which addresses the potential interaction effect between message framing and source credibility with a paricular focus on breastfeeding
behaviot
Breastfeeding has been widely recognized as the perfect source of nutrition for
babies for the first six months of their life.
The term "exclusive breastfeeding" refers to
the situation in which a baby is given only
breast-milk with no supplementary food (a1though medicines and vitamins, if needed,
conform to the definition). While it is esti-
mated that an
of 32 percent of
^verage
women around the wodd who exclusir.el),
breastfeed will do so for up to six months,
the proportion of Indonesian mothers $/ho
exciusive ly breastfeed is substantially less
than that figure. It fluctuates from below 20
percent to less than 10 percent since
1991 (Statistics Indonesia and Macro International 2008). These very modest figures suggest that devising a strategy to increase the
proportion ofwomen u'ho provide six months
exclusive breastfeeding could make a valuabie contribution to Indonesian's public
health. In addition, a better understanding of
the impact of health message framing in gen-
Gadiah Mada InternationalJournl
of B,tsness - May,4tg.rt,
t/01.
1
6, N0. 2, 201 4
erai could contribute to public health efforts
)oth there and elsewhere.
These gaps in our knowledge provide
'ustification for research that replicates and
estends prior studies in orderto guna deeper
:aderstanding of how to best stfucture per.uasive communications used for social marsering promotions tn alternative settings.
Based on the recognizedresearch issues,
study aims to detetmine the effect of
ctedibility on the
:.:titude and intention to provide six months
-sclusive breastfeeding. In addition, this
':-idv also looks in detail at the relationship
:-r\\:een attitude and intention. The results
, i rhis study are expected to contribute to
:'rth academic andpractical perspectives. For
--:-e academic perspectivg, this study will pro-.-rde an understanding about the effect of
::ressage framingand source credibility on the
.::itude and intention of people in the con:=rt of breastfeeding behavior. For the prac:cal perspective, this study will be the guide-res for social marketers and health practi:':ners to create a breastfeedingpromotional
:.npaign.
--::rs
:nessage framing and source
ing the desired behavior (I(eller
and
Thackeray 201,1).
Alden et al. Q01.1) suggest that social
marketing is different ftom ordinary health
education. Parkinson et.al. Q011) argue that
current breastfeedingpromotiors do not have
any effect in motivating women to breas tfeed.
In particular, they explain that people akezdy
have knowledge about breastfeeding and its
consequences. Hence, ordinary health education is not enough to influence individuals, to cafryr out this particular behavior.
In order to promote new health behavior, social marketers rely on promotion
(I(otler and Lee 2008). Specifically, promotion includes both the design and the delivery of persuasive communication in terms of
inspiring the target market to take a particular action. Stubblefield (1,997) identifies two
factors that seem to be particulady related to
persuasive messages, namely the characteristics of the source and the characteristics of
the message. Source factors telate to the person of orgxlzatton responsible fot the message andits distribution; meanwhile, message
factors relate to the characteristics of the in-
formation itself.
Research Model and
Hwotheses
Social marketing has been recognized
,. an important approach to formulate strat-::'e s to change health behaviot (Secket et al.
- ,'95). In terms of promoting new behavior,
:::-rrketing communication is an important
: -,mponent of social marketing (Alden et al.
-.,11). In the general context of social marce ung promotion strategy, social communi:.dons plzy an important role in describing
--:-- actions which the target audience should
::r. and in telling them the benefits of adopt-
Sevetal studies have shown that framed
messages have a significant effect on behav-
ior change (Abhyankar et aL.2008; Banks et
al. 1.995; Rivers c, ,1. 2005). With specific
tegard to influencjn lrealth behavior, paying
attentionto howa nlessage is framed has been
shown to be effective for altering attitudes
and subsequent behavior (Levin et al. 1998;
Rothman and Salovey 1,997; Rothman et al.
2006).In recognition of this, the manipulation ofmessage framing has been widely utilized in sociai marketing efforts.
The effect of message framing on
peoples attitudes and intentions can be ex-
95
Husseinet al.
plained by the Prospect Theory (Iversky and
I{ahneman 1981). The Prospect Theory ar-
havior with widely recognized benefits, it is
classified in this study as low elaboration be-
gues that people set an expected outcome as
a reference point. Any outcome beyond this
reference pointis considered a gain; and any-
havior.
thing less is viewed as a loss. The certainty
of reahzingan outcome increases the degree
to which people will seek to avoid a loss and
increases how attractiye g rn is petceived
^
to be (I{ahneman and Tversky 1'979). Therefore, a persuasive rnessage being framed either in terms of gains or losses should be relevant to the preferences for a nnge of behaviors. With the present focus, where a specific behavior -breastfeeding- is the focus,
the following hlpotheses can be generated:
H,:
Message
in
Hr:
bgea sfee
ding.
framing has an impact 0n an
indiuidual's intention to
engage
in breasfeeding.
Researchers have also shown that the
influential effect of a message on
an
individual's attitudes and intentions is moderated by the credibility of the message source
(A.rora and Arora 2004; Jones et aI. 2003;
Jones et zI. 2004).The current study uses the
Elaboration Likelihood Model (ELX,[) proposed by Petty and Cacioppo (1986) as abasis for examining the moderating effect of
source credibility on, first, the relationship
between message framing and attitude, and
second, between message framing and intention. The ELMexplains that, under low elaboratio n conditi ons, receivi ng information from
a credible source would be likely to provide
a cognitively easier route to accept the information given in a me ssage. In other words, if
someone tfusts the source and easily understands the facts of the issue, there is little
need to mofe thoroughly elabotate ofl, or debate, the contentions made in a message.
Given that bteastfeeding is a natural be-
96
credibiliry Olson
that the combi(1993)
suggested
andZanna
nation of a positively-framed message with a
highly credible source will be the most effective combination in promoting low elaborationbehavior Thereforg the credibility of the
message soufce should moderate the persuasiveness of the message on afl individual's
attitude and intention to provide exclusive
breastfeeding. Thus we propose that:
message framing and source
Hr:
There is a signtficant interaction
message
franing has an impact 0n an
diuid aa I's attita de toward
Message
In terms of the interaction between
ffict
between
Jraning and sowrce credibili\ on the
attitade to prouide exclusiue breastfeeding.
Given that breastfeeding is likely to be
alow elaborationbehavior, as argued above,
we expect that a positively-framed message
with a high credibilitysource will be the most
effective combination for affecting people's
attitude.
Hr:
There is a signrficant interaction
ruessage
franing and
intention
to
ffict
betueen
source credibiliry on the
prouide exclusiue breasfeeding.
Recognizing the Jinkage between attitudes and intentions, we expect that a positively-framed message with a high credibility
source will be the most effective combination for affecting people's intentions.
Several theories argue that attitudes
exhibit strong influence on, and thus can be
used to predict, intention. These perspectives
include the Theory of Planned Behavior
GPB) (Ajzen 1,991,), the Theory of Reasoned
Action GRA) (Ajzen and Fishbein 1980) and
theTriandis Model (friandis 1977). Ajzenand
Fishbein (1980) enhance the potential of
these theories by linking intention with behavior which is, ultimatell', a key outcome
Sflla&
&ure
Intemationallournt of hrsines -Mayfugat,
1. The Reseatch
W
I
6 I{0, 2,
Model
marketing efforts. In the realm of
ing behaviot, t\e hypothesized reb between attitudes and intention has
onfirmed through empitical work
et al. 1998; \ilfambach 1997). Thus
that:
/Wdc bas an ffict on a tuznan's intention
lrcl*riue! breasfeed.
These five hypotheses are graphically
as Figure 1.
Ate
201 4
sample of this study is pregnant
The selection of the group of pregwas based on the argument statthe decision to start breastfeeding is
rpde duringpregnancy (I(ramer et aI.
llertens and Young 1,997). Furtherirrstudies have shown that intetven-
tions such as peer suppoft andhealth education duringpregnancy will increase the likelihood of providing breastfeeding (Dyson et
21.2005; Ryan 1997).
The study relied on a convenience
sample drawn from a variety of locations.
Participants wefe rectuited viz a face-to-face
request made in several maternity hospitals,
obstetrici an clinics, and brea stfe eding workshops located in Malang, EastJava Province,
Indonesia. Three hundred and twenty questionnaires were distributed. Of these, 279
were completed, yielding a response rate of
87 percent.
The following demographic profile
emerged ftsn the sample: 68.1 percent of the
to30 years,
60.1 percent have completed a tefrary degree, 54.8 percent are working mothers, and
54 percent have more than one child. Table
1 presents the respondent's demographics
information
respondents areagedbetween
21.
Husseinet
aJ,.
Table 1. Demogtaphic of Sample Population
Category
Variable
Younger than20
Ag.
Education
Job
Number of Children (including in utero)
t
Measures of attitudes and behavioral
intentions were generated from the basic contentions of the Theory of Planned Behavior
(Ajzen1,991,; Duckett et al. 1998). The attitude toward exclusive breastfe eding wa s measured by six items using seven-point semantic differential scales. The scales utilized end
points of weak - strong, unpersuasive - persuasive, unimportant - important, unbelievable - believable, uninformative - informative, not convincing - convincing, and bad
afgument - good argument. Three seven-point
Likert scales were employed to measufe the
intention to exclusively breastfeed.
98
2.9
21
-30
68.1,
31,
- 40
27.6
41 -50
1.1,
High school graduate or less
36.9
Diploma
16.8
Bachelor's
40.1,
Mastef's
3.2
Working
54.8
Not Working
43
1,
46
2
39.1,
3
12.5
4
0.7
Over
Ques tionn afue Develop men
o
5
1.5
Design and Procedures
h 2 x 2 factonal design was used
to
measure the influence of message framing
and source credibility on a woman's intention to breastfeed. Since the design consists
of four factors, each discrete possible value
and all expedmental units take onall possible
combinations of these levels across all such
factors; the factorial design ptepared in this
study is considered as a full factortal experiment.
Four different adverti sing pamphlets
were designed to provide the unique combinations of positive and negative fnming as
well as low and high source credibility. Ma-
I
h Mada IntemationalJoutrnl of Brniner;s - Itfuy'4ryut, Vul I 6 Na
nipulation of the positively-framed message
uas done by presenting arguments that outfned the gains/benefits to be derived from
breastfeeding. In contrast, the negatively
2,
2U 4
ftamed m€ssage identified the benefits lost
by not engagingin the behavior Table 2 summarizes the framing manipulation used in this
study.
Trble 2. Messhge Ftaming Manipulation
Itoeitively Ftamed Me
ss
age
-rhat supplying exclusive breastfeeding
5r &e first six months -with continued
hasfeeding fo.tthe first year, ptovides sev-
cr{
benefits for both baby and mother.
Negatively Framed Message
...reported that not providing exclusive
breastfeeding for the first six months with
continued breastfeeding for the first year
could potentially te sult in serzet al dis advantages for both baby and mother.
hbaby:
For baby:
- fttrntially save 1.3 million lives
- Gmtaining all the vitamins and minerals
roded by a young baby,
- Lc'easing the level of antibodies
- Potentially lead to 7.3 million deaths every ye f
- Less chance to receive complete nutrition,
rihcing the chance of infection
- Hd a health digestion system
- Less eftctive antibodies
- Incteasing the chance of getting infec-
-
vitamins and minerals
tion
- Weakening
m&er:
*tcing pregnancies
Lecingthe chance to get ovananenGT
the digestive system
For mother:
- Less chance to
space pregnancies
- Having more potential to get ovaiancancer
Dccreasing the risk of breast cancer
lDrrrcrnigally cheaper than botde feed-
I
- Mote likely to get breast cancer
- Economically more expensive than
breastfeeding
Lrtry will have more chance of being
and suongand the mother will have
chance of avoiding many health probexclusive breas tfee ding ptovides
hcalth benefits compared with probotrle-feeding or other solid and liq-
... ababy will have less chance of being
hedthy and strongand the mother will have
less ability to avoid many health problems.
Providing botde-feeding or other solid and
liquid food potentially will not provide as
many benefits as six months exclusive
breastfeeding
Husseinet
aL
Table 3. Soutce Credibitty Manipulation
High Credibility Source
Low Credibility Soutce
B]': Dr Ana Sofi,vanti, M.I{es., Ph.D., SPA (I{ons)
By: Ana Sofiyanti
Undergaduate Student Faculty
of EconomicsJaya
Manggala
Universiry Jakafta
Regarding the message credibility r.'anable, the high credibility message was presented by a (fictitious) breastfeeding exPert
and also included a logo designed to represent a breastfe edingbased organization, v'hile
the 1ow credibility message was signed by a
university student and did not appear with
any organi:zati on's logo.
The tesearch stimulus was in the form
of a booklet, which contained instructions
and information relating to the study, message stimuli . and a questionnaire.
To begin the experiment, an information sheet was distributed when the potential participants ardved at the registtation
desk. After reading the information sheet and
agreeing to participate, the participants were
given brief instructions, asking them to read
the article and compiete the attached questionnaire. The participants were given one of
the four randornly selected stimulus messages.
Reading the article and completing the questionnaire took participants from 15 to 20
minutes. During the experimental session,
participantswere allowed to stop at any time.
In addition, participants were allowed to ask
the experimenter for assistance if they had
anv difficulties in reading and understanding
either the message stimulus or the questionnaire.
100
Data Analysis
Parttal Least Square regression (PLS)
was use d to analyze the data and test the hypotheses. Specificaily, Smart PLS with a
bootstrapping re-sampling procedure lvas
employe d. PLS was used since this technique
can handle both formative and reflective indicators. In this studl', ftaming and credibility are the formative indicators, whiie attitude and intention are the reflective indicators. Furthermore, the ability of PLS to ana
lyze data which have non-notmal distributions is anothet advantage. Finally, PLS has
the abilit1, to contfol measufement ertor and
can enhance testing of nomological webs
among muitiple dependent variables. PLS
thus is able to contribute to an improved experimental tesearch analysis in social sciences
(Streukens et al. 2010).
Forthis study, to model the experimental data with the framework of a structurai
equation, single indicators for the manipuiated factors, namely, message framing and
source credibilitv as well as the interaction
effect are specified. These manipulations utihze effect coding, such as a positively-framed
message is coded as 1, and a negativeh-framed message as 0, and a highly credible
source is coded as 1, and a lovr credible source
Gadiah Mada Internationauournal of Btrsiness -MayArqut,I'al. / 6,li,,. 2,
:s 0. Since the manipulations do not have the
:, pical propertie s of a reflective scale and it
:s assumed that the manipulation itself is
:re asured without error, this study depicts the
:rl :rnipul a tr ons a s formative indi ca tors. \X'hile
---re manipulations afe fepresented as forma-r'e indicators, the dependent variables - at:r.ide and intention afe fepresented as reflec:r-e indicators.
Manipulation Checks
Two tests examined whether the stimuli
:id any impact onperceptions. The first mea. -red the manipulation of credibilit,v. Parici:::rts wefe asked to fate the two versions of
--:r- message on six seven-point semantic dif:::ential scales, with the end points being
-: ustrvorthl' *not trustrirorthy, open minded
-:r,lr open minded, good -bad, expeft -not
:!)ert, experienced -not expefienced, and
-:.tred -not trained. The mean difference be-
I::le
20/
4
tween the high credibility source (HC) and
low credibilitl, sent.. (LC), where meanHc=
6.29 and meanlcr 4.13, was significant (p<
.001), indicating that the intended difference
in the message soufce's credrbility level was
perceived by the respondents.
A seven-point semantic differential was
also use d to measure the framingstimuli. This
use d end-points of positive
to rate
-negative
perceptions on how the message was framed.
The comparison of the mean scofes of the
positively-framed message (meanor.= 6. 78)
and negatively-framed message (mean^,.=
3.78), yielded a significant difference (p<
.001). This demonstrared that the rwo frames
were perceived as different bv the sample.
Results
Before testing the hl,potheses, it begins
with teliability, convergent validity and discriminant validity checks. Table 4 provides
4. Construct Loadings
Code
Factof
Loading
Atd_1
0.751
Atd-2
0.81 7
Atd_3
0.80
Atd*4
0.762
Atd_5
0.799
Atd_6
0.75
-{.rritude
---
rple a sant f Ple asant
I rbarrassing/Not
embarrassing
'-:,healthy
/Healthy
1-lu1sive/Appealing
/Con venie n t
-:
--on r-e nie n
-
rr-atural/Natural
t
1
B
lntention
-
:rpect...
Lrt_1
0.751
- ;--rnt. ..
Int_2
0.78 9
- -:lrend...
Int
0.729
.
3
..to provide my baby with six months of exclusive breastfeeding
101
Husseinet al.
With the outer model assessment indicating robust measufes, we then tested the
hypotheses. Findings indicate that message
framing does not have a significant main effect on eitherattitude (t= 1.534, p>0.05), ot
intention (t= 1.424, p> 0.05). This leads to
the rejection of both Hypotheses 1 and 2.
the PLS parameter estimates for the measurement model. The factorloadings for all scales
are
^cceptable,
indicating robust measures.
Table 5 explains the measures for examininginternal consistency of a given block
of indicators based on composite reliability
(Hair et aL.2010) and Average Variance Extracted (AVE) (Fornell and Larcker 1981).
The composite reliability for the constructs
tested exceed the cut-off value of 0.70 proposed by Nunnally.(l978). The AVE for the
constructs also exceeds the cut-off value of
0.
In terms of moderating effects, it
was
found that the source ctedibility significandy
moderates the effect of message framing on
attitude (t= 12.602, p< 0.01). This result
supports Hypothesis 3. Since there is a significant interaction effect between message
framing and source credibility on attitude,
Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) was used to
investigate the nature of this interaction.
Analysis indicates that the Estimated Marginal Mean (EMXO of a positively-framed
message coupled with a high source credibility is the highest @MNt= 5.756) comparec
to other framing and source credibility cornbinations (positively-framed message witi
low source credibility EMM= 5.236, negz
tively-framed message with high credibilir'
source EMM= 5.155 and negatively-ftamemessage with low credibility source EMII=
50 suggested by Fcrnell and Larcker (1 981 ).
Following Chin's (1 998) guidelines, two
methods were used to check discriminant
validity. The first was to compare the factor
loadings and cross loadings. To avoid having
a discriminant validity problem, the factor
loading coefficients should be higher than
their respective cross loadings. Table 5 indicates that the value of factorloadings foreach
construct is higher than the value of its respective cross loading. The second method
was to compare the value of square root of
AVE with the inter-correlation between constructs. Table 5 shows that the value of
square root of AVE is higher than the intercorelation among the constructs. Based on
these outer model evaluations, it can be said
that the measures used in this study are robust.
5.671). Thus, in terms
of influencin:
women's attitudes to provide six months -:
exclusive breastfee ding, a positively-fran:= :
message coming from a highly credible sou:r:
is the most effective combination. Horver--:
Table 5. Composite Reliability, AVE and Correlation among Consttucts
A\rE
Reliability
102
{^,ru
Composite
Intention
0.573
0.80
Attitude
0.611
0.904
1
Intention
Attitude
0.756
0.696
0.781
i
&4h
Mada IntemationalJouml of Bueiness -Iftayfuwt, Vul 6 M.
1
2,
2A4
rt is noteworthy that the combination of the
negaliysly-fiamed message with the low cre&
ilility
sorrce yielded an EEM impact on atrimde thatwas not substantially different from
positive-framed message, high credibility
lrxlrce EEM (5.756 versus 5.671,).
-'
fgure
2.
The significant moderating effect of
sorrce credibiJity was also found in the relatibnship between message framing and intention (t= 3,079; p< 0.01), supporringHypothesis 4. ANOVA was again used to determine
the nature of the interaetion, with outcomes
lnteraction between Framing and Source Credibility on Attitude and Intention
Estimatexd Marginal Means of Attitude
c
-Low
-Iow
crdility
crdility
c6
o
a
d
.s
o0
z
.o
x
q,
d
E
.E
lrl
Negatively-firamed
Positively-fnmed
Estimatexd Marginal Means of Intention
c
-Lwaodillr
-I.ow
d
o
a
d
c
'5n
!
a
X
o
d
E
'd
rI]
Neptivdyftrned
Positively-frrmed
edility
I
Husseinet al.
indicating that a positively-framed message
coming from a relativelv high credibility
source was the most effective combination
in affectingintention. The r.aiue of the EMM
for this combina,tion (5.910) is the largest
among the other combinations (positivelyfiamed message and low credibilitv source
EMM= 5.497, negativell-framed message
andhigh- credibiJity source EMNI= 5.333 and
negatively-framed and lou' credibility source
EN{NI= 5.835). Aswith the framingquestion
nation (R), message ftarningand the interaction of message framing and credibiiitv explarns roughly a quafter Q4.6Vo) of the variance in attitude. In addition, 50.8 percent of
the variance in intention was expiained b1'
attitude, messa€le framing and the interaction
of message framing and source credibility.
Table 6 summaizes the results of the hypoth-
results desc ribed above, the contf a st betrve en
eses testing.
the impacts of the positive-frame with high
credibility and negative-frame and iow credibiliw onintention were of comparable magnitudes (5.910 versus 5.835), F[ure 2 depicts
the interaction effect of message framing and
source credibilitl' on attitude and intention.
Hlpothesis 5's proporiiion that there is
a significant effect of attitude on intention
v/as supported by the data (t=I2.782; p<
0.01 , B = 0. 6 1 5). Attitude was shown to positivelv influence intention; the more positive
the attitude of rvomen towatd providing six
months of exclusive breastfeedirig the higher
their intention to exclusively breastfeed.
Referring to the coefficient of determi-
To ensure the robustness of the research
model, this study also uses the Goodness of
Fit (GoF) index. Since PLS is a variancebased SEN{, it does not harre a formal GoF
Therefbre, the calculation of GoF refers to
Tenenhaus et al's (2004) srygestion. Derived
from Tenenhaus et aI. (2004), Daryanle s1
al. (2010) proposed that the baselines forGoF
are small= 0.1, medium= 0.25 and large=
0.3(r.The GoF for this study (0.471) is quite
robust.
Table 6. Hypotheses Testing Summary
Hypothesis
104
Path
t-value
H1
Message Framing
-+ Attitude
1..534
NS
H2
Message Framing
-)
1.424
NS
H3
H4
MF * SC -> Attitude
MF * SC -+ Intention
H5
Attitude -+ Intention
Intention
1.2.602
Supported
3.07 9
Supported
12.782
Supported
'r:irh
Mada IntemationalJoumal of Busines - Jlay,4tgli,lhl. l6,l,o.2,2014
Discussion
The hlpothesized direct relationshrps
framing and attitude, and
and intention were not
nessage
framing
- .:rd. These results are inconsistent with
r-,se obtained in studies by Arora (2000,
: ,-) andJones et aL. (2004). The first pos.-:-e expianation for this difference is cen:.:ed on the nature of breastfeeding behavr.- :\\-een 1) message
:. \Iany campaigns have been launched to
:: ,rnote exclusive breastfeeding behavior in
-:::onesia; for example, in 2010 UNICEF
.
-:norted a mobile media-based advertising
:,:rpaign to raise awafeness of the benefits
- e rclusive breastfeeding for children. In
,::r:ion, and with respect to demographic
:--.r:cteristics, around 54 percent ofthe cur-=:-, study's participants haVe more than one
: --d. suggesting that breastfeeding and its
-:-.:ed issues afe not new to thern. Thus, it is
:.. -.ible that this study's participants were
,-:;;drof the benefits of exclusive
^ware
::=.stfeeding and, by recognizingthose ben.::'. already had a positive attitude toward
:::-;sive breastfeeding. These are attitudes
--cn could have been formed through prior
',,
:::, 'sure to similar messages, personal expe-:::ce or, perhaps, as a result ofgeneral soci::=- :uppoft of the practice. Therefore, there
. possibility that any stimulus provided
i . '-ci not have had anincremental effect on
:':--.-ious favorable evaluations of the prac-
:::The belief that they and their children
.:= certain to tezltze the benefits of
:;.s rteeding is another possible explanation
i ::re insignificant main effect of message
:.:.::::nq on women's attitudes towards pro' :-:: exclusive breastfeeding. If the argu-::-:s presented by the advertisement con-' ::: ro aheady held attitudes, it is feasonr -.: ro expect Little or no impact of framing
-
-"
on the attitude. The Prospect Theory
(I(ahneman and Tversky 197 9) posits that for
relatively lovz risk behaviors, gain-framed or
positivell.framed messages will be more persuasive than loss-or negatively-framed messagrs. Thus, iow risk behaviors can be thought
of as having little risk of not realizing the
s, do this and these
expected outcomes
-that
outcomes will occur" Altematively, although
breast milk has been clinically demonstrated
to be the complete nutrition fot infants
(Galson 2008), it is possible that message
framing alone was not enough to overcome
the thought that the outcomes of adopting
this behavior will vary between infants and
mothers. In addition, because positive outcomes are not easy to discern, perhaps thete
is an underlying concern that they will not be
attained.
Yet, when the positively-framed message is deliveted by a credible source, there
are differences found in both the attitudes
towards exclusive breastfeeding and the intention to engage in exclusive breastfeeding.
Perhaps while framing alone cannot dispel
perceptions of risk, the addition of a credible message source may lower to a degree
the possible uncertainfy that positive outcomes will be achieved and, consistent with
the tenets of ELM, make the message easier
to internalize.
Ftaming alone does not establish the
type of connection with the target audience
that framing in conjunction with a credible
message source appears to provide. If the
focus is shifted from the effect of external
stimuli, such as an advertisement that could
be use d in a social marketing campaign, and
looks instead at the psychologv behind the
motivation, or intention, to engage in a certain behavior, the Theory ofPlanned Behavior (TPB) can be used as a basis to understand this study's findings. Based on the TPB
105
I
Husseinet al.
(Ajzen L99I), an individual's intention to
adopt a desired behavior was affected by
three robust variables; attitudes, perceived
behavioral control (PBC) and subjective
norms. In accorda4ce to the TPB approach
and several studies in the area of
breastfeeding behavior(Dodgson et al. 2003;
Duckett et al. L998; Wambach 1997), this
study found that the women's favorable attitude toward providing exclusive breastfeeding
has a significant positive effect on their intention to exclusively breastfeed. This atti-
tude is unlikely to be something formed by
one exposure to an advertisement, but nther
it is formed over time and with repeated exposure to the stimulus, or as a result of different sources of influence pointing to the
same basic evaluation. Advertising can certainly serve as one of those sodrces tn alarger
social marketing campaign.
Based on the research findings, the most
significant theoretical contribution of this
research is the extension of the body of
knowledge by integrating ELM and the Prospect Theory in investigating the effect of
message framing and source credibility on
behavioral intention and attitude in the area
of social marketing. Specifically, the present
study provides a comprehensive evaluation
of the effect of message framing and the
moderating effect of source credibility on an
individual's attitude and intention to provide
exclusive breastfeeding behavior This study
validates previous studies such as Abhyankar
et al. (200 8), Jones et aI. (200 4) and O'Connor
et al. (2005), who found that message frzming does not have a significant main effect
on behavioral intention and attitude. Meanwhile, this study challenges the findings of
Jones et al. Q004) who investigated the effect of message framing and source credibility on behavioral interition and its predictors.
\X4rile Jones' study did not find significant
106
interaction effect berween message framing
and source credibilityon behavioral intention
and other socio-cognitive variables, this research found a significant interaction effect
where a positively-framed message coming
from a high credibility source was the most
effective combination in influencing behavioral intention and attitude. The combination
of a positively-framed message which comes
from a highly credible source supports the
prediction of the Prospect Theorl'
(I(ahneman and Tversky 1979), which suggested that an individual will avoid risk in
the domain of gains, and the ELM (Petty and
Cacioppo 19 86) which claimed that under the
low elaboration condition, a high credibility
source will be more persuasive than a low
credibility source.
Limitations and
Recommendations
The primary limitation of this study is
that it does not measufe the actual behavior
of interest; instead, it only measures the intention of women to exclusively breastfeed.
This limitation is common to many studies
utilizing intention as a dependant construct.
It is recommended that a future study could
move beyond measurement ofthis behavioral
proxy and could include a measure of actual
behavior. Another limitation is in regard to
the sample rectuitment. tJilhile this study obtained a relanvely large sample (n = 279), respondents were pfegnant mothers recruited
during a brief time span from several hospitals, matemity dinics and breastfeeding workshops in Malang, Indonesia. Hence the sampie
does not represent the population of pregnant Indonesians and the findings thus marnot be genera[zed to either non-pregnanr
mothers or pregnant women in other places.
Finally, since this study only investigated the
Gr&h fixda fnunationalJoutnal of hrsi'nas
-
May,&gast, Vll. I 6 M. 2, 2014
drect relationships among constfucts, any
frture study could also investigate indirect
knowledge about persuasive communication,
message framing, source credibility and be-
rdationships to provide more comprehensive
havioral change.
Refetences
ft*r.g
"tbhpnkar, P, D. B. O'Connor, and R. Lawton 2008. The role of message
vacciration: Evidence
of
in promoti4g
a loss-frame advantage. Pslcbokgy, Health and Mediciru 13 (1): 1.
Apeq I.1,991.. The theory of planned behavior. OrganiqationalBebauir andHumanDecision
21,7.
Process
MMR
50:1,79-
!
.!flren, I., and M. Fistrbein 7980. UnderstandingAttitudrs and Prediding Social Behauior. Prentice-Hall.
Jl5co, D. L., M. D. Basit and S. Destrpande. 2071. Communications in social marketing. In G. Hastings,
K. Angus, and C. A. Bryant @dt), The SAGE Handbuk of Social Marketing. London: SAGE
Publications Ltd.
Am,ra" R. 2000. Message framng and credibilityr application in dentd services Healtb Marketingpuarteill
18 (1-2):29.
fmr4
R. 2007. Message frami4g strat%ies for new and mature products
lomul of Produa
and Brand
Maragement 16 (6): 377-385.
drcre, R., and A. Arora. 2004.T1:r.. irnpact of message frami4g and credibility. SeruireMarketirgpaarterll
26 (1):35 -53.
"erora, R, C. Stoner, and A. Arora . 2006. Using framing and ctedibility to irrcoqporate exercise and fitness
in individuals' hfestty'e. Journal of Consumr Marketing % @: 199 - 207 .
fdqSM.,P.
of
Salovey,S. Greener,A,J. Rothman,A. Moyer,J.Beauvais,andE. Epel. 1995.The effects
message
Dqrafto, n., K.
regulatory
franirg on mammography utilization. Heahb Pgclnhgy
1,4: 1,78-778.
de Ruyter, and M. $Tetzels 2010. Getti4g a discount or sharing the cost The influence of
fit on corsumer response to service pricrg schemes lournal of Seryite Rsurch 13 Q):
153-167.
brfusorlJ.
E., S.J. Henly, L. Duckett, and M. Thrrant. 2003.Theory
of
planned behavior-based models
for bteastfeeding duration among Hong Kong mothers NarsingRcrearcb 52 p): M&
Eett,
L., S. Henly, M. Avery S. Pottet, S. Hills-Bonczyk, R.Huldeq and I{. Savik. 1998.
planned behavior-based strucn:rd model for breast-feedtng. NursingResearcb 47
A theory of
(6).
&mq
f
L., E McCormick, and M, J. Renfrew. 2005. Interventions for promoting the initiation of
breasdeeding. Cuhrane Database Slst Rn 2.
E#l,
C., and D. E Latcker. 1981. Evaluati4g strucural equation models with unobservable vadables
and measuremerit effor. lournal of Marketing resarch 18 (1): 39-50.
S K. E A). 2008. Mothers and childrenbenefit from breastfeedi4g. Jonrnalof tbeAnericanDietetic
Assuiation 108 (7): 7106 - 1707.
ElE,nfiC.Black,B.J.BabiaandD.R.Andetson 2010.MultimriateDataArafusirAGlo;batPerEatiue
Cf* .d). Upper Saddle River: Pearson Education Inc.
t07
HussehetaJ,.
Jones,
L. ![, R. C. Sinclair, and K. S. Courne va. 2003.The effects of source credibility and message
ftaming on exercise intentions, behaviors and attitudes: An integration of the elaboration likelihood model and prospect theory. Journal of Applied Socia/ Pychologt, 3J(1),1,79 - 1,96.
Jones, L. \V, Sinclak, R. C., Rhodes, R. E., and Courneya, K. S. (2004). Promoting exercise behaviour: an
integration of "persuasion theories and the theory of planned behaviour. Bitish jotu'nal of health
py ch ology, 9 (4), 505-521,.
I{ahneman, D., and A. Tversky. 1979. Prospect theory: An analysis
Journal of the Econometric S ocie!: 263-291' .
of
decision under risk. Econometrica:
I(eller, H., and R. Thackeray. 201 1. Social matketing and the cteative process Heahh Promotion Practice
1.2
(5): 651-653.
Kim, S.-8., and D-Y.'I<m.2014. The effecm of message framing and soutce credibility on green messages in hotels Cornell Hospitali4, puaterfi 55 (1): 64-75.
P., and N. R. Lee. 2008. SocialMarketing InfkncingBehaborsfor Good (3'd ed.). Thousan Oaks: Sage
PubLications
Kotler,
I{ramer, M. S., B. Chalmers, E. D. Hodnett, Z. Sevkovskaya, I. Dzikovic}r, S. Shapiro, J. P Colleg I.
Vanlovictql.MezeryandT. DucrueLnC1. Promotionof bteastfeedurg inerventbn trial (PROBIT).
J.4MA: thejournal of the American Medical Association 285 (4): 413-420.
I(umkale,G. T.,D. Albarraciqand P.J. Seignourel.2010. Theeffecs of source credibiliryinthepresence
or absence of prior attitudes: Implications for the design of petsuasive communication campatgns Journal of Appbed
S ocial Pslchology
a0 (6): 1325-1,356.
Leviq I. P, S. L. Schneider, and G.J. Gaettr- 1998. All frames arerrot created equal A gpology and cdtical
analysis of framing effects Organrqatnnal Behauior and Human Decision ProcessesT6 (2):149-188.
I{. Young. 1997. Determinants of breastfeeding in four CanadtanOJibwa commuA decision making model. Ameican Journalof Human Bnlogy 9 (5): 579-593.
Martens, P.J., and T.
nities:
Nunnally, l.
1
97
8.
P1 v ho me tri c
T h e or1. New York: M cGrav' -HilL
O'Connor, D. B., E. Ferguson, and R. C. O'Connor. 2005.Intentions to use hormonal male contraception: The role of mess€e framing, attitudes and stress appraisals Biti:h Journalof Pychology 96 (3):
351.-369.
J., andJ. D. Jensen. 2007.The relative persuasiveness of gain-framed loss-framed messages
encouraging disease prevention behaviors: Ameta-analytic review. Joarnal of Health Corumunica-
O'I{eefe, D.
for
tion 1.2 (7): 623
- 644.
Olson,J. M.,and M.P.Zaruw.1993. Attitudesand attitudechange. AnnualRwiew of Pgtchokgy 44(1):117-
{t
1,54.
Parkirsoq
#,
,-;
J., R. Russell-Bennett, and J. Previte . 201,1,.
aky. Aus tra lasi an
':
Mark
e
ting J o wr
na
Mum or bub? \X4rich influences breastfeeding loy-
I (AMJ).
Petty, R. E., and J. T. Cacioppo. 1986. The elaboration likelihood model
mmtal socialpgchologl 19 (1): 123-205.
of persuasion Aduanas
in experi-
Rivers, S. E,., P Salovey', D. A. Pizarrq J.P'aano, and T. R. Schneider. 2005. Message framing and pap test
utilization among women attending a community heahh clinic. /a wrnal af Health Pychobgy 10 (1): 65.
Rothman, A. J., and P Salovey. 1997. Shaping perception
message frami4g. Pslchobgical Bulleting 1.21 (1): 3 - 1,9.
108
to motivate
heahhy behavior: The role
of
Ga
diah Mada Intemationa{ournal of Busitess - May.Aryl t, W 1. 1 6, Nr. 2,
201
4
Rothman, A.J., R.D. Bartels,J. nflJaschiq and P. Salovey.2006. The strategrc use of gain-and loss-framed
messages to promote healthy behavior: How theory cafl inform practice. Journal of Communication
56 (s1):202-220.
I{. I(eougir, and C. D. Martin. 1993. The id-luence oi message
framing on intentions to perform health behaviors. Journal af Expeinntal SocialPg,.ho/agt 29 (.5):
Rothman, A.J., P. Salovey, C. Antone,
408-433. doi:
R1zn,
A.
1
0.1
006/ jesp. 1 993.1
S. 1997. The resurgence
01 9
of breastfeeding
in the United States. Pediatrics 99 @: e1.2-e12.
Secker, J., E. \7imbush,J. Watson, and I{. Milburn. 1995. Qualitative methods in
searclu some criteria for qualiry. Health E dacation lournal54 (1):74.
heakhpromotion re-
Statistics Indonesia, and Macro International. 2008. Indonuia Denographic andHealth Suruq 2007.MaryJand:
BPS and Macro International
Streukens, S., M. \feuels, A. Daryanto, and I{. De Ruyter. 20i0. AnalyAng factonal experimental data
using PI-S: An altemative approachand applbationin anonline complaining conto(t. InV EspositoVinzi W Chin, J. Henseler and H. Wand (Eds.) , Handbook of Panial l-.east Squares: Conceptq Methods
a n d App li c ati ors Springer.
Stubblefield, C.l,99l.Persuasivecommunication:Marketingheaithpromotion.l{ursingOutlook45:1.73171.
Tenenhaus, M., S. Amato, anfl
V Esposito Yirut2004. A global goodness-of-fitindex for PLS sffuctwal
equation modeiling. Paper presented at the Proceedings
of
the
XLII
SIS scienti{ic meetrng.
Triandis, H. C.1,977. Interpersonalbehailon Brooks/Cole Pub. Co.
Tr-erslq',A.,andD. I{ahneman. l98l.Theframingof decisionsandthepsychologyof choice.
Science,211.
(4481):453.
N-ambaclr, K. A. 1997. Breasdeeding intention and outcome:
Raearch in Nursing and Health 20 (I):51-60.
A test of the theory of planned behavior.
Zhang, Y, and R. Buda. 1999. Moderating fffects of need for cognition on responses to positively versus
negatively framed advertising messages Jowrnalof Aduertiing28 (2): I-15.
109