Hindi Speech Rate Effects and the Phonology of Voiced Aspirates For languages with two- and three-way stop contrasts, there is an asymmetrical effect of speaking rate on Voice Onset Time (VOT). As speakers slow down, prevoiced and aspirated stops are produced with more prevoicing and aspiration, respectively. Conversely, stops produced with short-lag VOT show little to no change in their VOTs (Summerfield 1981, Pind 1995, Magloire & Green 1999, Beckman 2014). Kessinger and Blumstein (1997) show that this asymmetry is found both in languages with two-way stop contrasts (e.g., English, French), and a three-way contrast (e.g., Thai). For Swedish, a language with a two-way contrast between prevoiced and voiceless aspirated stops (Helgason & Ringen 2007), Beckman et al. (2011) found more prevoicing and aspiration as speakers slowed down. They hypothesize that, in all of these cases, it is the phonologically-specified categories’ VOT values that undergo a change, assuming privative [voice] and [spread glottis]. The current study is the first to examine speaking rate effects in a language with a four-way stop contrast, Hindi, and the first investigation of rate effects on breathy voiced stops (voiced aspirates). Work beginning with Davis (1994), followed by Mikuteet & Reetz (2007) and Berkson (2012), adapted VOT so that for voiced aspirates it reliably captures both the prevoicing and breathy release. I examine how speech rate affects these Hindi stop cues, and then bring these phonetic data to bear on previous phonological accounts of voiced aspirates, for which authors have posited a range of features, varying both in number and type (Chomsky & Halle 1968, Ladefoged 1971, Davis 1994). Lombardi (1994) and Dutta (2007) note that a central issue is whether or not voiced aspirates should be phonologically represented via [voice] and [spread glottis] or whether breathy voice comprises an independent mode of phonation that should not have the same phonological representation as voiceless aspiration. I recorded 5 native speakers at different speaking rates, including data for the full four-way stop paradigm. Results show that for plain voiced stops and voiceless aspirated stops, both prevoicing and aspiration increase as a function of speech rate, respectively. The voiceless unaspirated stops show no changes in their VOT values. Crucially, both the prevoicing and breathy voice of voiced aspirates independently increase as speakers slow down. These results provide empirical support for a privative [voice, spread glottis] analysis of voiced aspirates, which naturally explains why the speech rate effects on the production of the post-release noisy period (breathy voice) of these stops are parallel to the changes found for aspiration of voiceless aspirated stops (e.g., English, German, Swedish). Moreover, such a phonological representation serves to form a natural class of aspirates, which is desirable based on phonological data from closely related Indic languages with four-way stop contrasts. As a result, this study fills a gap in the literature on speech rate effects, but also contributes empirical evidence relevant for a more accurate phonological analysis of voiced aspirates. 478 words References Beckman, Jill. (2014) Rate effects on VOT in German stops: Phonological implications. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Linguistic Society of America, Minneapolis, MN. Beckman, Jill, Pétur Helgason, Bob McMurray, & Catherine Ringen. (2011) Rate effects on Swedish VOT: Evidence for phonological overspecification. Journal of Phonetics, 39(1), 3949. Berkson, Kelly H. (2012) Capturing breathy voice: Durational measures of oral stops in Marathi. Kansas Working Papers in Linguistics, 33, 27-46. Chomsky, Noam, & Morris Halle. (1968) The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper & Row. Davis, Katharine. (1994) Stop voicing in Hindi. Journal of Phonetics, 22, 177-193. Dutta, Indranil. (2007) Four-way stop contrasts in Hindi: An acoustic study of voicing, fundamental frequency and spectral tilt. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Illinois at UrbanaChampaign. Helgason, Pétur, & Catherine Ringen. (2007) Regressive voice assimilation in Swedish. Proceedings of the International Congress of Phonetic Sciences XVI, 1357-1360. Saarbrücken, Germany. Kessinger, Rachel H., & Sheila E. Blumstein. (1997) Effects of speaking rate on voice-onset time in Thai, French, and English. Journal of Phonetics, 25, 143-168. Ladefoged, Peter. (1971) Preliminaries to linguistic phonetics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Lisker, Leigh, & Arthur S. Abramson. (1964) A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: Acoustical measurements. Word, 20, 384-422. Lombardi, Linda. (1994) Laryngeal features and laryngeal neutralization. Outstanding dissertations in Linguistics, Garland Publishing. Maglorie, Joel, & Kerry P. Green. (1999) A cross-language comparison of speaking rate effects on the production of voice onset time in English and Spanish. Phonetica, 56, 158-185. Mikuteit, Simone, & Henning Reetz. (2007) Caught in the ACT: The timing of aspiration and voicing in East Bengali. Language and Speech, 50(2), 247-277. Pind, Jörgen. (1995) Speaking rate, voice-onset time, and quantity: The search for higher-order invariants for two Icelandic speech cues. Perception and Psychophysics 57, 291-304. Summerfield, Quentin. (1981) Articulatory rate and perceptual constancy in phonetic perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7(5), 1074-1095.
© Copyright 2024 ExpyDoc