Lgcs 10: Intro Ling. Lecture Notes Monday, May 5th, 2014. (iii) Transformations (in the following order) 1. Final Class Grammar!!! Our grammar now has three components: V-to-T Movement Raise V to T, if T does not dominate a free morpheme. Condition: Only applies to Vcop. (i) A lexicon, which lists every word, along with its pronunciation, meaning, and word category. (ii) A set of PS Rules, which arrange words into sentences. Start symbol is S for declaratives, CP for interrogatives. The output of the PS Rules is called Deep Structure (DS). (iii) A set of ordered transformations, which take DS as input, and which move, delete, or insert nodes. The output of the transformations is called Surface Structure (SS). Each component looks as follows: (i) Lexicon N: kittens, squirrels, guys V t: like, love, devour, destroy, … Va: seem, look, … Vd2: serve, give, … Vcop: be A: nice, happy, brave… P: in, near, of, on, … Deg: very, pretty, quite, … D: the, a, those, these, some, your, … T: will, have, be, can, might, should, … Conj: and, or Pron: V i: Vd: Vc: she/her, he/him, they/them… smile, cry, yawn, … put, serve, give, … think, know… Adv: often, quickly, wisely, … PAdv: just, right, … C: that, whether, if, … Neg: not (ii) PS Rules S → NP T VP NP → {(D) AP* N (PP) | NP PP | Pron} VP → {Vi | Vt NP | Va AP | Vd NP PP | Vd2 NP NP | Vc CP | Vcop {AP | NP | PP} | Vp PP | AdvP VP | VP AdvP | VP PP | Neg VP} PP → {P (NP) | PAdv PP} AP → {A | Deg AP} AdvP → {Adv | Deg AdvP} CP → C S XP → XP Conj XP Heads are obligatory elements of a phrase. The head of a phrase determines the type and number of complements in a phrase (e.g., Vt requires an NP complement.) Complements occur as required by a head; they are always sister to a head. T-to-C Movement Raise T to Cq, if Cq does not dominate a free morpheme. VP Ellipsis (Optional) Delete a VP that is identical to a preceding VP in the context. Tense Hopping Lower T to V, if T does not dominate a free morpheme. Condition: Cannot cross Neg. Do-Support Insert the dummy auxiliary do to bear bound (stranded) tense morphology. 2. V-to-T Movement Summary. Across languages, tense morphology, whether bound or free, is generated in T. When a tense morpheme is free, e.g., will or have, nothing happens. When a tense morpheme is bound, e.g., [PRES], [PAST], a number of ensure that the bound morpheme combines with a verb stem: In French: V-to-T Movement for all verbs. In English: V-to-T Movement for Vcop; otherwise Tense Hopping, otherwise, Do-Support. Our evidence for positing V-to-T Movement comes from sentences in which T does not dominate a free morpheme. In such sentences, we find the following word orders: (a) V Adv Object; (b) V Neg Object; (c) V precedes the subject in yes/no questions, if the language uses T-to-C Movement to form yes/no questions. 1 3. Yes/No-Questions Summary. We arrived at the following generalization for the formation of questions across languages: Yes/No-questions are CPs headed by a Cq, where Cq must be occupied by a free morpheme at surface structure. To achieve this, in some languages, Cq is occupied by a lexical complementizer, while in other languages, T moves into Cq by a rule of T-to-C Movement. In both cases, a free morpheme must appear in Cq. 4. Updated Summary of Cross-Linguistic Comparisons Headedness refers to the order of heads and complements in a language. Languages tend to be head-initial or head-final (but are sometimes mixed). Head Movement refers to transformations that move one head to another (e.g., V-to-T Movement, T-to-C Movement.) Ellipsis refers to transformations that delete nodes (e.g., VP Ellipsis). Yes/No-Questions. Yes-No questions are generated as CPs headed by Cq, where Cq must be occupied at surface structure by either (i) a Cq from the lexicon, as in Japanese and Hebrew, or (ii) T-to-C Movement, as in English and French. 5. Pidgins and Creoles When speakers of mutually unintelligible languages come into contact and need to communicate, a pidgin language can develop. Pidgins are rudimentary, limited languages: they have a small vocabulary, and a simplified grammar. Pidgins usually arise in a situation that involves at least three languages, one of which is dominant. The dominant language usually contributes most of the vocabulary; this is called the superstrate language. The other languages are called substrate languages, and can provide the phonology of the pidgin. Pidgins do not have native speakers; rather, speakers of a pidgin maintain their native language but use the pidgin to communicate with other groups. Over time, a pidgin can develop into a creole, a full-fledged language with native speakers. This is believed to result when the children of speakers of a pidgin develop it into a complete language. When a pidgin becomes a creole, its vocabulary and grammatical system expand dramatically. Creoles are examples of language genesis. Comparison of languages considered so far: French: Headedness: V-to-T Movement: Yes/No Questions: VP Ellipsis: Hebrew: Headedness: V-to-T Movement: Yes/No Questions: VP Ellipsis: Japanese: Headedness: Yes/No Questions: English: Headedness: V-to-T Movement: Yes/No Questions: VP Ellipsis: Head-initial Yes T-to-C Movement No (1) Comparison of creoles (From Bickerton 1985, “Creole Languages”) Hawaiian Haitian creole Sranan creole base: he walked he walk li maché a waka anterior: he had walked he bin walk li té maché a ben waka irreal: he will/would he go walk l’av(a) maché a sa waka walk nonpunctual: he is/was he stay walk l’ap maché a sa waka walking he would have walked he was/had been walking he will/would be walking he would have been walking he bin go walk he bin stay walk he go stay walk he bin go stay walk li t’av(a) maché li t’ap maché l’av ap maché li t’av ap maché a ben sa waka a ben e waka a sa e waka a ben sa e waka 2 6. Non-Configurationality (adapted—at times word-for-word—from Baker 2001) English and French are said to be configurational in the sense that subjects and objects consistently occur in certain positions within a tree, and verb and object form a constituent to the exclusion of the subject. Non-configurational languages do tend to have rich morphological case and agreement systems, suggesting at least that there are two types of typologically distinct languages: non-configurational, which rely on morphology to express syntactic relations, and configurational, which rely on phrase structure. Not all languages are configurational in this sense. Nonetheless, many researchers argued that even non-configurational languages have hierarchical structure, based on certain familiar English-like asymmetries between subject and object. For example, in the Australian language Warlpiri, any order of subject, verb, and object is possible, as long as the auxiliary occurs in the second position in the clause (data from Hale 1983): For example, in English, an object may be anaphoric to a subject, but not vice versa: (2) (3) Ngarrka-ngu ka man-ERG PRS.IPF ‘The man is spearing the kangaroo.’ wawirri kangaroo panti-rni spear-PST Wawirri Panti-rni Ngarrka-ngu Panti-rni Wawirri panti-rni ngarrka-ngku panti-rni warirri ngarrka-ngku ngarrka-ngku warirri warirri ngarrka-ngku panti-rni ka ka ka ka ka Subjects and objects appear to have the same syntactic distribution, and there is no evidence that the verb and object form a constituent to the exclusion of the subject. Given facts like these, flat structures have been proposed in which subject and object are not distinguished by phrase structure configurations. Any language in which it is difficult or inappropriate to use phrase structure to distinguish grammatical functions like e.g. subject and object could be called nonconfigurational. The class of languages that have been called nonconfigurational includes most Australian languages (Dyirbal, Nunggubuyu, Jiwarli, Jingulu); various American Indian languages (Salish, Uto-Aztecan, Iroquoian, Algonquian, Klamath/Sahaptin/Nez Perce); certain South American languages (Quechua); various New Guinean languages; South Asian languages such as Malayalam; Hungarian, Japanese, perhaps even German). (4) a. The two old men are looking at themselves. b. *Themselves are looking at the two old men. Similar facts hold for Warlpiri (Hale 1983): (5) a. Purlka-jarra-rlu ka-pala-nyanu old.man-Dual-Erg Pres-3DualS-Reflex ‘The two old men are looking at themselves.’ paka-rni. see-NonPast b. *Purlka-jarra ka-nyanu-palangu old.man-Dual Pres-Reflex-3DualO ‘Themselves are looking at the two old men.’ nya-nyi. see-NonPast …the question of how to fit non-configurational languages into linguistic theory is relevant to some of the deepest issues of linguistics—including the questions of how much variation Universal Grammar allows and what are its proper primitives (phrase structure, grammatical functions, or something else?) (Baker 2001, p. 8) References Baker, M. 2001. The Natures of Nonconfigurationality. In Baltin, M. and C. Collins, Eds., The Handbook of Contemporary Syntactic Theory. Blackwell. Legate, J. 2002. Walpiri: Theoretical Implications. MIT PhD dissertation. 3 7. French vs. Haitian Creole The following sentences illustrate negation in French. Is the position of pas 'not' in French as expected? (13) tonga taorian’ ny mpamboly arrived after the farmer ‘The old teacher arrived after the farmer.’ (6) (14) namono ny akoho tamin’ ny antsy killed the chicken with the knife ‘The woman killed the chicken with the knife.’ Nous avons pas we have not ‘We have not seen John.’ vu seen Jean. John (French) (7) Nous voyons pas Jean. we see not John ‘We don’t see Jean.’ Consider now Haitian Creole, one of Haiti’s two official languages (along with French.) (8) Bouki déjà konnen Bouki already knows ‘Bouki aready knows Boukinet.’ Boukinèt Boukinèt (Haitian Creole) (9) Bouki pa konnen Bouki not knows ‘Bouki doesn’t know Boukinèt.’ Boukinèt Boukinèt (Data from Carnie 2006) ny mpampianatra antitra the teacher old ny the vehivavy woman (15) nanaseho sari n’ Noro ny lehilahy ny reni n’ ny zaza showed picture of Noro the man the mother of the child ‘The mother of the child showed the man a picture of Noro.’ (i) Provide PS Rules for S, NP, AP, VP, and PP in Malagasy. You may leave out T in your S rule, since there is no evidence for this position in these data. (ii) Is Malagasy head-initial or head-final? (iii) Provide the trees for the sentences above, given your rules. (iv) How does Malagasy differ from English? 9. More practice with English Provide derivations for each of the following, given our final class grammar: Does Haitian Creole have V-to-T Movement? How can you tell? Derive (9). 8. Practice with PS rules in another language Malagasy is an Austronesian language spoken throughout Madagascar. Based on the following data, answer (i)-(iv) below. (10) namangy anay ny visited us the ‘The children visited us.’ ankizy children (11) mihinana ahitra eat grass ‘The cow eats grass.’ omby cow ny the (12) matory ny mpamboly sleep the farmer ‘The farmer(s) is/are sleeping.’ (16) (17) (18) (19) (20) (21) (22) (23) (24) Are you never at home on Fridays? We asked if we could put some things on your bed quietly. Does it look okay? Have you seen Kimiko? Do you often see Kimiko? Do you? (Meaning: Do you often see Kimiko?) Is she? (Meaning: Is she Kimiko?) They talked to Kimiko on Friday. We did not. (Meaning: We did not talk to Kimiko on Friday.) Revise the PS rules so that they account for the following sentence, and then provide a tree for the sentence, given your revision. (25) (26) Whether it will rain again is uncertain. That you are well-prepared seems likely. 4
© Copyright 2024 ExpyDoc