Note: this pre-print version contains some minor technical problems, e.g. the devoiced diacritic may not display properly. PRINMI: A SKETCH OF NIUWOZI* Picus Sizhi Ding 1 INTRODUCTION Spoken by the Pumi and the Tibetan nationalities in northwestern Yunnan and southwestern Sichuan, Prinmi (or Pǔmǐ in Chinese transliteration) belongs to the Qiangic branch of the Tibeto-Burman languages (Sun 1982; Bradley 1997: 35– 37). With widespread settlements in the mountain areas, speakers of Prinmi live close to many communities of distinct Tibeto-Burman languages. Lu (1983: 90) proposed two major dialectal groups: Northern Prinmi and Southern Prinmi. Ding (2014: 8–10) further divides the latter into two, resulting in a tripartite grouping: Northern Prinmi, Central Prinmi, and Western Prinmi. Mutual unintelligibility of varying degrees exists among these dialectal groups. Niuwozi Prinmi, the variety presented in this chapter, belongs to Central Prinmi. It is spoken in central Ní nglàng YíAutonomous County in Yunnan, adjoining Sichuan. 2 PHONOLOGY 2.1 Syllable structure Unless treated as an independent segment, the glide is considered as part of the nucleus of syllable. This avoids overloading the consonant inventory (but see Matisoff 1997 for a different approach). An analysis of the syllable structure is illustrated in Figure 36.1. 2.2 Vowels, glides, and consonants Niuwozi Prinmi has fifteen monophthongs. These are given in Table 36.1 according to their height, frontness, roundedness, and nasality. After a fricative or an affricate, /ɨ/ is realized as a fricative vowel homorganic with the preceding consonant, e.g. sɨR[s ] ‘to die’, tʃhɨR [tʃh ] ‘dog’, and ʐɨF[ʐʐ] ‘four’ (superscript letters indicate surface tones of different tonal categories: R = rising, F = falling, and H = high). The vowel /ɨ/ also contrasts, in non-fricative form, with its rounded counterpart, e.g. bɨH ‘sun’ versus bʉH ‘(of grain) heap’. Phonetically /y/ is realized as [ɥi], but /ỹ/ is a true monophthong. Discounting /y/, /ỹ/, and /ĩ/, high and mid high vowels are prone to lowering, hence /i/: [i]~ i], /o/: [o]~ o], and o fort . Preliminary cross-dialectal comparison suggests that /ɜ/ is historically related to *ε. This central vowel contrasts with schwa, e.g. mɜF ‘bamboo’ versus məF ‘ ky’. Niuwozi Prinmi has three glides: /j/, /ɥ/, and /w/. The latter two are almost in complementary distribution but contrast in such minimal pairs of native words as kɥɑH ‘melon’ versus kwɑH ‘(of eye) to open’. Treating a glide as part of a rhyme leads to a number of rising diphthongs. All three glides can function as the on-glide in diphthongs, but only /j/ and /w/ may serve as the off-glide: ju, jɜ, jɑ, j , jõ; ɥe, ɥε, ɥa, ɥ ; wɜ, wa, wɑ, wɑ ɜj, aw; wɜj Table 36.2 displays all consonants in Niuwozi Prinmi. All of them, including sonorants, contrast in voicing. Further, the various types of oral stop show distinctive aspiration. The rhoticized stops invovles a special secondary articulation (Ding 2009). Major allophones of consonants in Niuwozi Prinmi include a set of uvular plosives which are allophones of the velar set before /ɑ/; t e palatali ed fricative [ʝ], an allophone of / / before /i/ or /j/ (not applicable to /ʃ/); the bilabial fricative [ϕ], an allophone of /x/ before /u/; and the optional palatalization of /n/ to [ɲ] before /i/ or /j/. * The description of Prinmi presented here is based on fieldwork done in Nínglàng County, Yunnan. I am indebted to many Pǔmǐ people for their invaluable help. Except for the suprasegmental notation, Prinmi data are presented in IPA according to the latest revision of 2005. Abbreviations – ASR: assertive; BNY: beneficiary; CAUS: causative; CLF: classifier; CLC: cislocative; COP: copula; DUR: durative; DSC: discourse clitic; DS.N: desiderative negator; EXT.AN: (animate) existential; FRM: frame-setting marker; INS: instrumental; IPFV: imperfective; M: modificatory; MRT: mirative; NMLC: nominal clause marker; NMLZ: nominalizer; PF.N: perfective negator; PFV: perfective; PL: plural; PL.IN: inclusive plural; Q: question; QUOT: quotative; SG: singular; TLC: translocative; TOP: topic marker; and VLT: volitive. C36 p. 1 TABLE 36.1 MONOPHTHONGS OF NIUWOZI PRINMI Front High Mid high Central oral i y nasal ĩ ỹ oral e Back ɨ ʉ u o nasal õ Mid ə Mid low ɜ Low oral ɑ a nasal TABLE 36.2 CONSONANTS OF NIUWOZI PRINMI Plosive Bilabial Dental b p pʰ d t tʰ Fricative Nasal m m Lateral z s n n l l Post-alveolar Retroflex ɡ k kʰ ʃ Rhotic Affricate Rhoticized stop dz ts t ʰ Velar d tʃ tʃʰ ʐ ʂ ɹ ɹ ɣ x ɖʐ ʈʂ ʈʂʰ ɡʴ kʴ kʰʴ bʴ pʴ pʰʴ 2.3 Suprasegmentals Prinmi morphemes are predominantly monosyllabic. Monosyllabic words may bear a high tone, a falling tone, or a rising tone; e.g. bj H ‘bu y’, bj F ‘urine’, bj R ‘to fly’. The suprasegmental contrast between the high tone and the falling tone is difficult to perceive in monosyllables (Ding 2001; Ding 2007a). This is due to the fact that a monosyllabic domain is too short to manifest the underlying contrast between them. However, when words are followed by clitics in connected speech, these three tones effectively contrast in two pitches: high versus low. The falling and rising tones are often split into a H-L and a L-H sequence respectively in longer domains, whereas the high tone gives rise to H-H (see Table 36.3 for details). The suprasegmental patterns observed with longer domains indicate that Prinmi tones are differentiated along two parameters: (a) the location of the H tone on the underlying quadrisyllabic prosodic domain (which covers the basic scope of a tonal category), and (b) whether the H tone spreads to the successive syllable. The combination of these two settings yields a total of seven categories: TABLE 36.3 SUPRASEGMENTAL CATEGORIES IN NIUWOZI PRINMI Category Parameters Quadrisyllabic Trisyllabic Disyllabic I 1] − pread] H-L-L-L H-L-L H-L II [1] [+spread] H-H-L-L H-H-L H-H III 2] − pread] L-H-L-L L-H-L L-H IV [2] [+spread] L-H-H-L L-H-H L-H V 3] − pread] L-L-H-L L-L-H L- VI [3] [+spread] L-L-H-H L-L-H L- VII [4] [spread] L-L-L-H L-L- C36 p. 2 Monosyllabic L (Falling) H (High) (Rising) (Rising) Tonal patterns found in verb morphology point to both Tone III and Tone V as the sources for the rising tone in monosyllabic verbs, e.g. bj R ‘to fly’, ɡəL-bj H ‘fly away’, ɡəL-bj H=siL ‘flew away’ versus sɨR ‘to die’, nɜL-sɨR ‘die’, nɜLsɨL=siH ‘died’. The basic tonal pattern of each category, as specified by the two parameters in the table, remains unchanged on each row. The marked tone, H appearing on the syllable indicated by the first parameter, is essential to a tonal category, but other tones following it are subject to truncation, as the length of the prosodic domain becomes shorter. These tonal categories form a phonological system similar to that in Japanese (Ding 2006), which is termed the ‘melody-tone system’ in Ding (2007a), to distinguish it from the better known syllable-tone languages of East and Southeast Asia on one hand, and from the word-tone languages of Nepal (as discussed in Mazaudon 1977: 76–84) on the other. 2.4 Phonological alternations Phonologically-conditioned segmental variation is relatively few, but ‘tone and i’ i perva ive in Prinmi. Most tone changes ensuing from differences in the prosodic domains where a morpheme is situated are predictable. However, extraprosodicality may occur in prefixed verbs and numeral-classifier compounds (see Ding 2014: 60). Phonological processes which affects lexical tones include: (a) shifting of the H tone to an adjacent syllable (usually to the right), (b) reversal of the H-spreading feature, (c) merger of two prosodic domains, and (d) split of a prosodic domain into two. 2.5 Intonation Intonation exerts a great influence on the surface tones of lexemes. This is achieved mainly by merging two prosodic domains in an intonational phrase, as in (1), where the word having lost its lexical tone in the merger proccess is underlined. It is also possible to insert a H boudary tone to the right edge of an intonational phrase, after the original tone is deleted and replaced by the default low tone; see (2). (1a) | ʐ H toL=boL | tʃɨH=ɡeH sheep on= FRM water=TOP ‘On the sheep, water is heavy.’ lɜjL | heavy (1b) | tɜH iL=boL | koLjiL=ɡeL one:day=FRM crow=TOP ‘Then one day Crow came over.’ ɡəL-tʃʰ F | CLC-appear (2) tʃɨR % | ɡəL-ʐɑH | water out-carry ‘the water washed (him) away.’ (tʃɨH: H L+H%=R) | 3 MORPHOLOGY 3.1 Lexical categories Prinmi lacks derivational morphology that changes merely lexical categories without introducing additional meaning. There is considerable overlap between noun, verb, and adjective. Monofunctional adjectives such as buLlɑH ‘many’ and guHʒĩL ‘(adjective) middle’ are scarce. Incidentally, the latter has a noun form guLʒĩH ‘(noun) middle’; t is is the only minimal pair with an identical meaning contrast in tones. As a rule of thumb, when a simplex non-grammaticalized word modifies a noun postnominally, it functions as an adjective, often in a noun-adjective compound. Many adjectives can also be used as nouns, verbs, or both. Eleven minor lexical categories have been identified, including (no suprasegmental marking for toneless morphemes): existentials – ʃiR ‘(ab tract t ing ) to exi t’, ʒeF ‘(animate) to exi t’, dʒĩF ‘(animate) to exi t among a group’, djõF ‘(inanimate) to exi t’, kɥeF ‘(inanimate) to exi t wit in omet ing’, and tɜjF ‘(inanimate) to exi t on omet ing’; auxiliary verbs — e.g. kʰuR ‘mu t; need’, ʒj H ‘can’; demonstratives — e.g. təH ‘t i ’, əHtəH ‘t at’; pronouns — e.g. ɜH ‘I’, neR ‘you ( ingular)’, neLdz H ‘you (dual)’, eLɹəH ‘we (inclu ive)’, ɑLɹəH ‘we (exclu ive)’, niF ‘ e/ e/it (visible to interlocutors)’, ts H ‘ e/ e/it (invisible to interlocutors)’; numerals — e.g. tiR ‘one’, niR ‘two’, s R ‘t ree’; C36 p. 3 R classifiers — e.g. tsɨ ‘(for uman )’, b ‘(for tree )’; ideophones — e.g. tʃjatʃja ‘an inten ive tate of w itene ’, ʂ ʂ ‘an exce ive tate of abundance’; adverbs — e.g. tiLtiH ‘ lowly’, lɜLlj onomatopes — e.g. kʰ L· tʰ L R ‘very’; ‘ ound of weig ty object falling into deep water’; interjections — e.g. wiF ‘expre ing an unplea ant urpri e’; and postpositions — po ‘under’, to ‘on’, khʉ ‘on top of’, wu ‘in ide’, lo ‘out ide’, dʒe ‘at one’ place’, be ‘at; to; from’ (also ‘wit (in trumental)’, ni ‘wit (comitative)’, bo ‘for marking the semantic roles of locative, goal, and source), (benefactive)’, and ki ‘(beneficiary marker)’. Many postpositions are grammaticalized from nouns. The ongoing process is changing these words into clitics; the fully cliticized instrumental can form composite variants with other clitics, e.g. ɹõ< ɹə (plural clitic) + õ. 3.2 Affixes Suffixes are more common than other affixes in Prinmi. They impart additional meanings to a word, e.g. pɜL-tsɨH ‘flower’; tsawH-dĩH ‘beating implement’; tʰj L-jiH ‘beverage’; ɹa LʒawH ‘to be prone to laug ing’. Antonyms of some descriptive verbs/adjectives are derived with the negative prefix, e.g. mɑL-ʂ H ‘unclean’. Most verbs can be affixed with at least one of the six directional prefixes given in Table 36.4 (cf. Fu 1998: 27–72 for verb prefixation in Lánpí ng Prinmi): TABLE 36.4 THE DIRECTIONAL PREFIXES Category Person Prefix Meaning dɜ-/dəh Prefix h Meaning cislocative (towards the speaker) t ɜ- translocative (away from the speaker) Space gə-/k ə- outwards (x)ɜ- inwards Vertical nɜ- downwards tɜ- upwards There are two unproductive nominal affixes: the vocative prefix ɑ- and the conjunctive infix -mə-, e.g. ɑL-kawF ‘materal uncle’; nĩH-mə L-nĩ L ‘day after day’. 3.3 Inflection A small number of verbs and auxiliary verbs in Niuwozi Prinmi are inflected for agent agreement in terms of person and number, as exemplified in Table 36.5.1 The inflections are often associated with the perfective aspect but their occurrence is variable. TABLE 36.5 THE CONJUGATION OF THE COPULA AND VERBS Root First person singular dzɨF dj pɑF tʰj R maLsɨF Second person singular 1/2 person plural Third person Meaning F dawF dĩF dzɨF be pj F puF pĩF pɑF do tʰ R tʰ tʰĩR tʰɥ maLsĩF maLsʉF maLsj F R maLsawF R drink know Reciprocal verbs are derived through reduplication, e.g. tsɜHtsawL ‘to fig t’ < tsawH ‘to beat’; ɡjɜLɡeR ‘to elp eac ot er’ < ɡeR ‘to elp’. Notice the modification of the vowel in the first syllable. A few verbs are remnants of an archaic causative prefix of Proto-Tibeto-Burman. They contrast with the base form in terms of controllability and voicing, e.g. kʴj H ‘to break (something)’ ver u ɡʴj H ‘(something) to break’; pheF ‘to de troy’ versus beF ‘to collap e’. C36 p. 4 3.4 Compounding Compounding is extremely productive in Prinmi. Except for those permitted in specific patterns, affixes and clitics do not partake in compounding. Most compounds are bisyllabic or quadrisyllabic; trisyllabic ones typically involve ideophones, e.g. bɨH L L ‘paper-t in’ and pʰʴĩHtʃjaHtʃja L ‘snow-w ite’. Iterative compounding results in complex structure. For instance: {[(kʰʴəHɹuH) ɹɜLkɑL][(loLtʃʰɥeH) ɹɜLkɑL]} ‘fibu ’ foot:stem bone out:side bone 3.5 Clitics Clitics are attached to phrases rather than words. Except for negative and interrogative clitics, all others are enclitics. Major nominal clitics include the plural ɹə and the dual dz , e.g. tʃj LɹəH ‘c ildren’ and kɹhəHdz H ‘two feet’. Ot er nominal clitics mark topics (see Section 4.9) and modificators (see Section 4.2). They can cling to a VP occasionally. Discourse clitics such as bo and ne can be considered to be a minor type of nominal clitic. They function much like gap fillers and can be readily omitted (as witnessed in editing recorded spontaneous speech). Verbal clitics are richer than nominal ones in number and form. The experiential tʉ signifies experience of doing something. The durative nõexpresses a continual state; cf. (21) and (28). The hortative ɡi conveys the wish to engage in some activity together. The quotative tʃɨɹju is used for marking hearsay information whose truth is not committed to by the speaker, e.g. (3). Its precise function in (3b) is to mark mirative, a psychological sense of surprise (see DeLancey 1997). (3a) (3b) mjaHbuF=ɡeL=boL kʰʴəHɡ L toL nɜL-ʈʂwɑL=siH eyelid=TOP=FRM knee on down-contact=PFV ‘It i aid t at t eir eyelid touc down to t e knee .’ pʉHʃiL last.night ɜH 1SG ʒɨHmeL tiL dream one tʃɨL=ɹjuL QUOT ɡəL-tʃʰ L out-appear ɜH x L tiH beH tʰɜL-pʴawF=siL *(tʃɨL=ɹju) 1SG parrot one to TLC-transform=PFV MRT ‘ a t nig t I dreamt of my elf tran forming into a parrot.’ TABLE 36.6 ATTACHMENT PATTERNS OF THE INTERROGATIVE AND THE NEGATIVE CLITICS Clitic group Attaching position Clitic type Bare monosyllabic verb ɑ/mɑ/me/tjɑ + Verb Proclitic Bare disyllabic verb Verb' + ɑ/mɑ/me/tjɑ + Verb Endoclitic Prefixed, no other clitics Prefix + ɑ/mɑ/me/tjɑ + Verb Mesoclitic With other verbal clitics (Prefix) + Verb + ɑ/mɑ + Aspect/Modal Enclitic Aspect, modality, and evidentiality can be expressed with sets/pairs of clitics sensitive to the factor of controllability. Of the two perfective markers si and sj , the basic one is si; see (9) and (15). When an activity is controllable by the agent, sj is chosen (for first person singular). Likewise, the pair of modal clitics, ʃo and kɜj, differ in whether or not the wi to do omet ing i under t e agent’ direct control; cf. (25). Controllability also plays an important role for the set of imperfective clitics ɹõ, ɹu, and ɹĩ, versus the default one ɹju; see (10) and (12). The former set conveys controllability of a situation by interlocutors, whereas the latter indicates a lack of such control. The variants of the set for constrollable situations are selected according to the person and number of the agent: ɹõ(first person singular), ɹu (second person singular), and ɹĩ (first/second person plural). Occurring at the end of the utterance, attitudinal clitics usually bring in some extra information about t e peaker’ attitude or emotional state at the time of speaking. Commonly seen attitudinal clitics are: ɡjɑ (expressing surprise), mɑ (making a suggestion), pɑ (speculation), and mə (making an assumption). Of these, mɑ and pɑ are likely to be loans from Southwestern Mandarin. Prinmi has three specialized negative clitics: the general negator mɑ, the perfective negator me, and the desiderative negator tjɑ (often found, but not exclusively, in imperative sentences). These negators and the interrogative clitic ɑ are the only ones that may precede the verb host, as they prefer the penultimate position in the clitic group. Depending on the number of syllables in the clitic group, the negator and the interrogative clitic may appear as a proclitic, an endoclitic, a mesoclitic or an enclitic, as summarized in Table 36.6. C36 p. 5 4 SYNTAX 4.1 Grammatical system Prinmi does not operate on a grammatical system with a clearly definable concept for subject and object. Although unstable agent agreement is found on a small number of verbs, no other grammatical properties suggest the existence of subject in the language. Prinmi tends to mark the agent with the instrumental clitic õ, especially after a third-person singular noun in the form of ɡõ; cf. (14) and (23). Sometimes this optional marking is also found after non-third-person agents. The inconsistency suggests that the development of ergativity in Prinmi is at an early stage (cf. LaPolla 1995). 4.2 Structure of the noun phrase Six kinds of modifying elements may appear in a Prinmi noun phrase. According to their closeness to the head noun, the noun p ra e can be analy ed a aving four layer ( ee Figure 36.2): T e ‘modificator’ i a pecial type of modifier, typically signified by the modificatory clitic ɑ or its complex variants such as ɡjɑ. Genitive expressions, e.g. ɜH=ɡjɑH kʰʴəH ‘my foot’, and the relative clause, exemplified in (4), are effected through the modificatory relation. The modificatory clitic is often omissible; cf. the genitive expression in (6). The following contains all the possible elements in a noun phrase, except for the postposition/clitic: (4) [mjaHbuLtɜjL] eyelid:big [tʃɨLdĩH flood diH-mi=ɑL] cast-er=M təH this ʃɨHbɑH əLɡj H ʃɨHbɑH puLdiL nɜHtsɨL dragon:family old.man dragon:family old.woman two:CLF ‘these two big-eyelided old man and old woman of the Dragon family who have started the flood’ The pair of coordinate heads (in boldface) in (4), each taking an attributive noun, is qualified by two modificators (placed within brackets), a demonstrative, and a numeral expression. 4.3 Structure of the clause The structure of Prinmi clauses can be described elegantly with the layered analysis advanced in Role and Reference Grammar (see van Valin and LaPolla 1997 and references therein). The smallest layer is the nucleus, containing the predicate. The core layer consists of the nucleus and the core argument(s) of the predicate. Modifiers of the predicate such as temporals and locatives are situated in the periphery. These three together form a clause expandable to a simplex sentence, as shown in Figure 36.3. Within the scope of a clause, word order is quite rigid, starting from the adjunct/modifier in the periphery, followed by the core argument(s) on the core layer and then the verb on the nucleus layer. The default order of arguments is agentrecipient-theme, but core arguments can also appear outside the clause when they serve as sentential topics. Note that the extra clausal positions are reserved for pragmatic topics; simultaneous topics at both ends are prohibited. Sentence-initial topic is dominant while sentence-final one marginal. For instance (bracketing indicates the layers within a clause; N = nucleus, C = core, and P = periphery): (5) neR P[ɹəLbeH]P C[ʈʂĩLʈʂuL=ɡeH N[nɜL-diL nɜL-ʃɨH keH.]N ]C 2SG first pestle=TOP down-cast down-go caus ‘You t row t e pe tle down fir t.’ The presence of the temporal expression ɹəLbeH ‘fir t’ in (5) is helpful for recognizing a left-detached argument. For the core argument neR ‘you’ to precede t e temporal at the periphery, it must be situated at the left-detached position outside the clause. 4.4 Major sentence types Exemplified in (6)–(8) are the major sentence types: declarative, negative, and interrogative. (6) pɜHdiH ɜH ɖʐuL dzɨH. frog 1sg friend cop cop ‘Frog i my friend.’ (7) mɜHtʃeH ɹiR meL=thõH. seek get PF.N=succeed ‘( e) earc ed (but) couldn’t get (one).’ C36 p. 6 (8) põHpõL ɑH= eL? uncle q=ext.an ‘I uncle ( ere)?’ Prinmi also has tag questions which imply a pragmatic presupposition. These are constructed by rendering the interrogative clitic as the tag, with its own intonation phrase, as shown in (9). An information-seeking question arises when an interrogative pronoun is employed in situ, without any change of word order, as given in (10); see also (16). (9) neR dɜL-jiH=siL ɑL? 2sg clc-come;2sg=pfv q ‘You’ve come over, aven’t you?’ (10) neR hɜHkiL ʃɨL=ɹuL? 2sg where go=ipfv;2sg ‘W ere are you going?’ Verbs in imperative sentences generally show consistent agreement with the agent, even though it is often expressed implicitly. Negation of imperative sentences requires the use of desiderative negator, as in (11b). (11a) (neL) dzɑwH. 2sg eat;2SG ‘(You) eat.’ (11b) (neLɹəH) tjɑL=dzĩH. 2pl DS.N=eat;2PL ‘(You) don’t eat.’ 4.5 Clause compounding When two Prinmi verbs are adjacent in a sentence, they may conjoin into a unit: t e ‘double-verb predicate’ (t e term ‘double’ i not to be taken literally). On account of recursion it is possible to have more than two verbs in this type of complex predicate. As with morphological compounds, the conjoining is binary in nature and it occurs on the nucleus layer. The verbs in the complex predicate are syntactically interdependent on each other, but without subordination. A prime example of double-verb predicate (double-underlined) is provided in (12). Note that ʃɨH ‘go’ function a an auxiliary verb and takes the clause headed by the double-verb predicate as its complement. (12) ts LɡyH=ɡeH niF beL ɡəL-jɜR tʰɜL-kʰj L ʃɨH ɑL=ɹjuL? attire=TOP 3SG to out-fetch TLC-give go q=ipfv ‘Will (t e Golden P ea ant) go bring and give t e clot e back to im?’ (13) ts LɡyH=ɡeH ɡəL-jɜR niF beL tʰɜL-kʰj L ʃɨH ɑL=ɹjuL? attire=TOP out-fetch 3SG to TLC-give go q=ipfv ‘Will (t e Golden P ea ant) go bring t e clot e and give (t em) back to im?’ Given in (13) is another type of clause compounding. The erstwhile shared argument (single-underlined), intervening between the verbs, has prevented formation of a double-verb predicate. Notwithstanding its identical meaning to (12), (13) takes the syntactic structure of clause-chaining sentence, similar to the following: (14) məF=ɡ L=neL məF toL bɨH ɜH-tyL liH ɜH-tyL. sky=ins=dsc sky on sun in-put:3 moon in-put:3 ‘T e eavenly god placed a un and a moon in t e ky.’ Clause-chaining sentences involve conjoining of clauses at a level higher than the nucleus. They are looser in structure and laxer in prerequisites, and thus occur more commonly than the double-verb predicate in Prinmi. 4.6 Subordinate clauses Clause subordination is found in relative clauses and periphrastic constructions where an embedded clause serves as a complement to the auxiliary verb. Sometimes verbs of cognition may also take a complement clause. Prinmi uses the nominal clause construction to convey a sense of discovery based on cursory observation of the surroundings. This construction consists of a relative clause and the marker ti, which functions as the structural head. For instance: (15) təH miF=ɡeL nɜL-sɨL=si=ɑH tiL. this person=TOP down-die=PFV=M nmlc ‘T i per on (i ) dead.’ (occurring in a direct peec ) C36 p. 7 As with other relative clauses, the modificatory clitic in (15) is omissible. When the copula is used in periphrastic constructions, its complement clause is nominalized by the suffix -ji for the obligational construction, e.g. (16), or by the suffix -mi for the focus-presupposition construction, e.g. (17). (16) meH tʰɜL-tʃʰɨL-jiL dzɨL? what tlc-do-nmlz cop ‘W at ould (we) do?’ (17) ʃj H=ɡ H gəL-tsɑwH-miH dzɨL. iron=INS out-hit-NMLZ cop ‘(T e tool,) w at I’m aying i : (it) i forged from iron.’ Complement clause to auxiliary verb is not marked by any grammatical means, and this frequently leads to juxtaposition of two verbs, e.g.: (18) kwɜjH=boH tjɑL=kwɜjH kuH . cry=frm ds.n=cry must ‘A for crying, (you) mu tn’t cry (any more).’ (19) [[[ eLɹə H=boL ɖʐəL ɖʐuL tʃʰɨH]3 ɹ HɹĩL=ɑL=ɹjuL ]2 ɜL-ʃjaL ʃjaH]1 1pl.in=frm friend do want=Q=IPFV in-try ‘(You) give a try and ee if ( e) want to make friends wit u .’ tiL one puL do;2SG That the head verbs of these complements do not constitute a double-verb predicate with the auxiliary verb is evinced in the negation pattern in (18) and subordination in (19). When a double-verb predicate is negated, the negative clitic appears on the final unit, as in (20). However, the negator occurs within the complement clause in (18). (20) neR tɜH-pʰeH tʃʰɨL ɡəL- ɨR mɑR=keL=boL 2SG up-cough.out do out-come N=CAUS=FRM ‘If you don’t coug ( im) out …’ (lit. ‘cough up – don’t let come out’). Three orders of subordination are observed in (19). Of these, only the head of the inner most complement is adjacent to an auxiliary verb. The other instances see intervention of a clitic between two verbs of cognition and separation of the auxiliary verb from its complement by the numeral ti ‘one’. Non-embedded subordination is rare. Adverbial clauses of manner may involve such subordination, e.g.: (21) niFɹəL thɜL-ʃuLʃuH=nõL tʃhwɑLt ɜjH pɑL=nõL kɹõLkɹiH. 3pl TLC-gather=DUR voice:big do=DUR sing ‘T ey ing aloud toget er.’ Both adverbial clauses in (21) happen to contain the durative clitic nõ, but it does not signal subordination. 4.7 The causative construction Prinmi has various means for expressing causation. The most important one is with the causative verb, built on the following structure: Causer + Affectee/Goal/Sociative + Complement clause + Causative verb ke According to the degree of volitionality and/or affectedness, the causee can be expressed as a sociative, a goal, or an affectee, as provided in (22)–(24) respectively (cf. Shibatani and Pardeshi 2001). Irrespective of how the causee (rendered in boldface in the examples) is encoded, it is always co-referenced with the agent in the complement clause. (22) ɜH neL kiH ɡoR poL 1sg 2sg bny hill below ‘I let you go down t e mountain.’ nɜ L-ʃɨH down-go (23) ʃj F=ɡ L kiHpuL beL nɜL-ɡwɜjH keL. golden. pheasant=INS cuckoo to down-dress caus ‘T e Golden P ea ant got t e Cuckoo to put (t e clot e ) on.’ (24) niHɹ L ɜH ɜ L-ʃɨH meH=kɥ H. 3pl;ins 1sg in-go pf.n=caus;3 ‘ I wanted to go in;] t ey wouldn’t let me in.’ C36 p. 8 keF. caus (Affectee) (Sociative) (Goal) When a powerless causee is involved in a direct causation, it is encoded as affectee without marking by any postposition, as in (24). In a situation where the causee could have avoided the event, it is marked by be as a goal in the indirect causation, as in (23). Finally, the causee may be a willing participant in an event; in such cases, the causee is signified with ki in the sociative causation, as seen in (22). 4.8 The comparative construction The comparative construction has the following structure: Theme + Noun phrase/Clause + to + Descriptive verb Strictly speaking, the comparative construction lacks morphosyntactic marking. The intended comparison is achieved only when the postposition to ‘t an (literally: on)’ i con trued a a sort of complementizer which introduces the standard for comparison. This is often a noun phrase, but it can also be a short clause. In transitive sentences, the comparative construction may be embedded in a relative clause to modify the patient argument. An instance of this is provided in (25), where the comparative construction is underscored. (25) ɜH nɜ LɡeH təH dj LbaH toL tʃhɥɑH tiL=boL mɜHtʃeH pɹhəR mɑL=kɜjH. 1sg again this place than good;M one=DSC seek meet n=vlt ‘I won’t be able to find a place better t an t i one again.’ (lit. ‘ eek not meet one better t an t i place’). 4.9 The topic–comment construction Prinmi has two topic markers: the frame-setting clitic bo for establishing discourse scenes, typically regarding a locative frame (see (1a) above) or a temporal frame, e.g. (26), and the general topic clitic ɡe for indicating a topic on which a comment is made. Note that ɡe is often used to mark specificity rather than a topic; see (1b) above, where koLjiR ‘crow’ i newly introduced to t e di course. On the other hand, a topic can also occur without any morphosyntactic marking; see (6). Major types of topic-comment construction include the double-topic construction, chained comment construction, and embedded topic-comment construction. The first type may be simplex in structure, but the other two are always complex. The double-topic construction consists of a frame-setting topic (double-underlined below) and a topic (single-underlined) in an aboutness relation with a comment (placed inside brackets), e.g.: (26) mɑLtoH kʰeH=boL boR, ɹɜLpuF=ɡeL [nɜL-diL]. final time=FRM DSC rooster=TOP down-cast ‘Finally, the rooster (you) throw (it) down.’ In the chained comment construction a topic is shared by two or more comments in chained clauses, e.g.: (27) lʉHb L=ɡeL [sj Lb H tɜHt ʰ L], [bĩH], [lɜLlj R ɡɥ H]. China.fir=TOP tree one:kind radially.thick very tall ‘T e C ina fir i a kind of tree, radially t ick, and very tall.’ The topic–comment relation may be recursive, giving rise to the embedded topic-comment construction (cf. Ding 2007b: Chapter 7). The outer topic and the embedded topic in the construction must hold the semantic relation of set–member. For instance, in (28) the sentence initial topic represents a whole and the other topics body parts. In (29), the set–member relation is realized as agent-and-activity. The set denotes activities done by the agent. This semantic relation is necessary, but not sufficient, for topic-comment embedding. Note that the complex structures of (28) and (29) involve not only topic-comment embedding but also chained comments. (28) j LdzɨHpɑLpɑL [ zʉL=ɡeH [ ɖʐɜL=n L]] [ ʈʂoL=ɡeH bat face=TOP bad=DUR organ=TOP ‘T e bat, (it ) face i ugly; (yet it ) organ are perfect.’ (29) ʈʂɜH earth [dziL]]. wonderful to=ɑL ɖʐɜLɖʐɜH tʃhɨH miL=boL on=M bad do person=FRM [ tsiHlɜjH=boL [wuLɖʐɑH khəL-tʃhjõL j L]], seed:sow=FRM harvest:poor out-appear asr [ ɡĩLʃawH=boL [ɖʐõR mɑR=j L]], livestock:raise=FRM smooth n=asr [ t ʰ LtʃʰɨH lɑH [kuHdziH ɹiR mɑR=j L]]. business:do also profit get n=asr C36 p. 9 ‘Per on w o behave badly on earth, for cultivation, a poor harvest will certainly come; for livestockrai ing, (it) certainly won’t be moot ; for doing bu ine , profit certainly won’t be made.’ REFERENCES Bradley, David (1997) ‘Tibeto-Burman languages and classification’, in D. Bradley (ed.) Papers in Southeast Asian Linguistics: Tibeto-Burman Languages of the Himalayas, Canberra: Australian National University, 1–72. Caughley, Ross (1982) The Syntax and Morphology of the Verb in Chepang, Canberra: Australian National University. DeLancey, Scott (1997) ‘Mirativity: t e grammatical marking of unexpected information’, Linguistic Typology 1: 33–52. Ding, Picus Sizhi (2001) The pitch-accent system of Niuwozi Prinmi. Linguistics of the Tibeto-Burman Area 24.2: 57–83. Ding, Picus Sizhi (2006) A typological study of tonal systems of Japanese and Prinmi: towards a definition of pitch-accent languages. Journal of Universal Language 7.2: 1–35. Ding, Picus Sizhi (2007a) The use of perception tests in studying the tonal system of Prinmi dialects: a speaker-centered approach to descriptive linguistics. Language Documentation and Conservation 1.2: 154–181. Ding, Picus Sizhi (2007b) Studies o Bǎ Resu t tive Co structio : comprehe sive ppro ch to M d ri Bǎ se te ces, München: Lincom. Ding, Picus Sizhi (2009) Rhoticization as a secondary articulation in stops: Evidence from Prinmi. Chinese Journal of Phonetics 2: 74–81. Ding, Picus Sizhi (2014) A Grammar of Prinmi: based on the central dialect of northwest Yunnan, China, Leiden: Brill. Fu, Ailan (1998) Pumiyu Dongci de Yufa Fanchou (Grammatical Categories of Pumi Verbs), Beijing: Chinese Cultural and Historical Publisher. LaPolla, Randy (1995) ‘Ergative marking in Tibeto-Burman’, in Y. Ni i, J. Mati off, and Y. Nagano (ed ) New Horizons in TibetoBurman Morphosyntax, Osaka: National Museum of Ethnology, 189–228. Lu, Shaozun (1983) Pumiyu Jianzhi (An Outline of Pumi), Beijing: Nationalities Press. Lu, Shaozun (2001) Pumiyu Fangyan Yanjiu (A Dialectal Study of the Pumi Language), Beijing: Nationalities Press. Matisoff, James (1997) ‘Dayang Pumi p onology and adumbration of comparative Qiangic’, in S. Prem rirat et al. (eds) Memorial Volume for Paul K. Benedict (Mon-Khmer Studies vol. 27), Bangkok: Mahidol University, 171–213. Mazaudon, Martine (1977) ‘Tibeto-Burman tonogenetic ’, Linguistics of the Tibeto-Burman Area 3.2: 1–123. Shibatani, Masayoshi and Pardeshi, Prashant (2001) The causative continuum. In M. Shibatani (ed.), The Grammar of Causation and Interpersonal Manipulation, Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 85–126. Sun, Hongkai (1982) ‘On t e ubgrouping of t e Qiangic language ’ (in C ine e), in Minzu Yuwen Yanjiu Wenji, Xining: Qinghai Nationalities Press, 189–224. van Valin, Robert and LaPolla, Randy (1997) Syntax: Structure, Meaning, and Function, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. FIGURE 36.1 THE STRUCTURE OF PRINMI SYLLABLES FIGURE 36.2 A LAYERED ANALYSIS OF THE NOUN PHRASE FIGURE 36.3 THE STRUCTURE OF THE SIMPLEX SENTENCE C36 p. 10
© Copyright 2025 ExpyDoc