Phonology Forum 2014 High Vowel Devoicing in Japanese: As an Indicator of Standardization of Dialect PhD Lecture 日本語狭母音の無声化 ー共通語普及の指標としてー Hi-Gyung Byun Akita International University August 20th / The University of Tokyo, Komaba Contents • Introduction • Purpose of the study • Methodology (materials, acoustic analysis) • Results (Inoue Data, Takada Data) • Discussion (standardization of dialect) 2 Introduction • Vowel devoicing in Japanese • Distribution map of vowel devoicing • Issues regarding the current map • Resolving the issues 3 INTRODUCTION Vowel Devoicing in Japanese • In Japanese, there is a regional difference in occurrence of vowel devoicing. For example: • Tokyo Japanese devoices a high vowel between voiceless consonants with high frequency, while Osaka Japanese devoices with very low frequency in the same condition あきた a k i̥ t a devoiced vowel [ i̥ ] by Tokyo Japanese Speaker あきた akita voiced vowel [ i ] by Osaka Japanese Speaker • The map of vowel devoicing (Hirayama, 1960; Nihon Hoso kyokai, 1966, 1998) shows the regional difference 4 INTRODUCTION Distribution Map of Vowel Devoicing The ONLY map of vowel devoicing Prominent areas of vowel devoicing Non-prominent areas of vowel devoicing Hokuriku Izumo Chugoku Shikoku Kyushu Chubu Northern Tohoku Prominent areas • • • • • • Southern Tohoku Kanto Some parts of Chubu Hokuriku Izumo Kyusyu Southern Tohoku Kanto Kinki NHK Japanese language pronunciation and accent dictionary (Nihon Hoso Kyokai, 1966, 1998) Non-prominent areas • • • • • Northern Tohoku Many parts of Chubu Kinki Chugoku (except Izumo) Shikoku 5 INTRODUCTION Issues Regarding the Current Map • This map is not made by investigating under a consistent methodology throughout the country (Sawaki, 2005) • The border lines are not clear, and the map needs to be made up based on the words with the same sound environments (Mase,1977) Some more issues… • No criteria for dividing into two areas, prominent vs. non-prominent (When we say “prominent,” how often does vowel devoicing occur?) • No explanation for differences within the same (non) prominent areas (Is a high vowel devoiced in the same condition for Kanto and Kyushu areas? Usually, Kanto area often devoices a high vowel in word-medial position when a devoiceable vowel is not accented; while Kyusyu area often devoices a high vowel in word-final position, irrespective of the accent ) • If the map is based on Kindaiti’s (1954) study, the data do not show the current status of vowel devoicing (Standardization of dialects (common Japanization) has progressed for several decades and is still progressing, so we can easily imagine there will be some changes regarding vowel devoicing) 6 INTRODUCTION Some issues Resolving on the thecurrent Issues map • It is almost impossible that an individual investigates the entire country by oneself (Sato, 1985) • Recently, large-scale speech databases have been open for research purpose • This study made use of two different, large-scale, speech database covering most parts of Japan and various age ranges • One was collected between 1986 and 1988 from 41 prefectures that included 607 participants (high school students and their grandparents), ⇒Inoue Data (It is appropriate to examine regional and generational differences) • The other was collected between 2006 and 2007 from 7 areas that included 463 participants ranging in age from 14 to 90 ⇒Takada Data (It is a follow-up investigation to Inoue Data, and best for investigating age differences) 7 PURPOSE Purpose of the Study The purpose of this study is • to comprehensively examine vowel devoicing emphasizing on regional and age differences as well as phonetic conditions, and • to provide new insights into vowel devoicing in Japanese Based on the result from devoicing rate by each age group in each area, this study will suggest • a new distribution map that reflects current status • three stages of change of devoicing rate 8 PURPOSE Purpose of the Study (continued) Furthermore • we will verify that synchronic generational differences in devoicing rate is a sound change in progress • and for some areas, we will identify the times that devoicing rate is changed dramatically We will also discuss • social factors for change of devoicing rate in some areas, and • the future devoicing rate by applying the current devoicing rate of five areas to an S-curve model 9 Methodology • • • • • • Inoue Data Takada Data Investigation points Target words Sound environments of target words Acoustic analysis 10 METHODOLOGY Inoue Data • A part of large-scale speech corpus collected by support of KAKENHI from 1986 to 1988 (Inoue 1989) • All utterances were recorded on analog tapes by high school students and sent to the principal investigator • 22 words which include a high vowel between voiceless consonants were selected from 607 participants in 41 prefectures and converted to digital format, and then analyzed acoustically • The number of participants from Tohoku to Kyusyu by generation: a rea Aomori Aki ta total 7 8 15 7 7 14 a rea Shi ga Na ra old 3 3 6 2 2 4 old young young total Iwa te Ya ma g Mi ya gi Hukus h Gunma Tochi g Iba ra ki Chi ba Sai ta m Ka na ga Ni i gata Naga n Yaman Is hi ka Hukui 7 8 15 8 8 16 14 12 26 22 22 44 10 11 21 11 13 24 7 7 14 3 3 6 7 8 15 4 8 12 Os aka Wa ka y Hyogo Tottori Okaya Hi ros h Ya mag Ka gaw Tokus h Kochi 3 3 6 3 3 6 8 8 16 1 1 2 17 17 34 4 1 5 1 1 2 17 17 34 8 7 15 8 8 16 16 15 31 6 6 12 Ehi me Hukuo 11 11 22 13 14 27 3 3 6 4 4 8 Oi ta Sa ga 4 4 8 8 8 16 Gi hu Ai chi Shi zuo Mi e 4 4 8 2 3 5 6 8 14 2 2 4 14 14 28 Naga s Kuma m Mi ya z Kagos h tota l 4 4 8 5 5 10 Hokkaido and some remote islands were excluded. No data in Tokyo, Kyoto, Toyama, and Shimane 5 5 10 10 12 22 299 308 607 11 METHODOLOGY Takada Data • A part of large-scale speech corpus collected by support of Hakuho Research Grant between 2006 and 2007 (Takada2009) • The recordings were made face-to-face for each participant using a computer to digitally record utterances • 34 words which include a high vowel between voiceless consonants were selected from 463 participants in 7 areas and analyzed acoustically • The number of participants for each area by age group: area Akita Tochigi Ibaraki Tokyo 23 wards Tama district Osaka Hyogo Awaji Island Kumamoto total Hyogo 60's 18 12 11 6 3 1 2 2 9 64 50's 13 7 10 15 1 7 5 2 10 70 40's 11 5 6 4 2 6 4 1 5 44 30's 13 2 7 5 2 5 8 2 5 49 20's 13 4 18 18 10 8 19 1 16 107 10's 17 11 28 17 8 20 19 2 7 129 I participated in the recording of Akita, Ibaraki, Tokyo, Osaka, and Hyogo total 85 41 80 65 26 47 57 10 52 463 12 METHODOLOGY Investigation Points • Inoue Data ○ Takada Data 13 METHODOLOGY Target Words • Utterances in isolation • Phonetic condition is word-medial vowel devoicing, which a high vowel is devoiced almost regularly in Tokyo Japanese (average devoicing rate is over 80%). The target words are the words that include a high vowel between voiceless consonants and the high vowel has no accent kernel • Word-final vowel devoicing, which a high vowel comes between a voiceless consonant and a pause, is NOT the condition that a high vowel is devoiced regularly (Byun 2007) 65 64 56 53 Devoicing rate (%) in word-final position 44 41 40 40 38 38 36 35 34 31 31 31 29 26 26 24 22 21 21 21 20 20 18 18 17 17 16 16 15 15 15 14 13 12 10 3 2 Hiroshima Nara Kagoshima Osaka Hyogo Saitama Yamanashi Yamaguchi Tottori Tokushima Ishikawa Wakayama Iwate Kochi Nagano Shiga Ehime Akita Nagasaki Kagawa Miyagi Oita Okayama Hukuoka Hukui Saga Hukushima Gihu Yamagata Ibaraki Shizuoka Niigata Aomori Tochigi Mie Gunma Kanagawa Chiba Aichi Miyazaki Kumamoto Average of 10 words あき(秋),えき(駅),ひらく(開く),まつ(松),でぐち(出口),しゅじゅつ(手術),あいさつ(挨拶),るす(留守),りす(栗鼠),なす(茄子) METHODOLOGY Sound Environments of Target Words Inoue Data Takada Data (22 words) (34 words) following consonant following consonant affricate くし(串), くすり(薬) affricate ちち(父), ちち(乳) つくる(作る) つち(土) ふたり(二人), ふた(蓋), ポスト ふく(服), たすき(襷) ふし(節) plosive きく(菊) きさく(気さく) くさ(草) くち(口), くつ(靴) ちから(力) ちくわ(竹輪) くし(串) ごちそう(ご馳走) ちちおや(父親), ちつづき(地続き) みちすじ(道筋) つかれ(疲れ) つくり(作り) fricative だいきち(大吉), きつえん(喫煙) れきし(歴史) くとう(苦闘) くき(茎) fricative した(した) preceding consonant くさ(草) ちから(力), ちかい(近い), おちた(落ちた) fricative preceding consonant れきし(歴史) くち(口), くつ(靴) affricate あきた(秋田) きしゃ(汽車) plosive きた(来た) plosive fricative affricate plosive あつさ(暑さ) つち(土), つつみ(包み) うつし(写し) しかた(仕方) たのしさ(楽しさ) しくみ(仕組み) しちじ(七時), しつれい(失礼) オアシス ふたり(二人) ふさく(不作) ふく(服) ふち(淵), ふつう(普通) ふし(節) target following vowel vowel i i u u i i u u i i u u non-high high non-high high non-high high non-high high non-high high non-high high : no words The underline represents a target syllable (preceding consonant + target high vowel). The crossed out words ちつづき and ふち were excluded from the analysis due to a problem in recording. 15 METHODOLOGY Acoustic Analysis fully voiced vowel partially voiced vowel devoiced vowel 2% or so on average for each area • • • fully voiced vowel: several periodic waves and several glottal pulses on the spectrogram partially voiced vowel: quasi-periodic waves and weak glottal pulses on the spectrogram devoiced vowel: no waves and no visible glottal pulses for a vowel on the spectrogram devoicing rate (%) = the number of devoiced vowels / target vowels ×100 16 Main Results • Description for each area and comparison with the previous study • New distribution map of vowel devoicing from Inoue Data • Three stages of change of devoicing rate from Takada Data • Synchronic generational differences of devoicing rate from Takada Data 17 RESULT Result • The details of the devoicing rate for each area are described by generation or age groups in terms of five phonetic factors: preceding consonant, following consonant, target high vowel, following vowel, and word accent ⇒⇒ ⇒⇒ Example of description of each area The description part takes up two-thirds of the dissertation. I will not mention the details here Here, we focus on the results below: • New distribution map • Three stages of change of devoicing rate • Synchronic differences of devoicing rate as a diachronic change 18 RESULT New Distribution Map Current map Inoue Data old generation (Birth year is 1930 and older) Devoicing rate is over 60% especially prominent or regularly devoiced Prominent areas of vowel devoicing Non-prominent areas of vowel devoicing Estimated percent is used for the area with no data NHK Japanese language pronunciation and accent dictionary (Nihon Hoso Kyokai, 1966, 1998) 19 RESULT New distribution map (Inoue Data) Old generation Young generation (Birth year is 1930 and older) (Birth year is between 1967 and 1973) Clear generational and regional differences! 20 RESULT Three Stages of Change of Devoicing Rate abstract model Initial stage Middle stage ・High vowels between fricative and plosive, fricative and affricate, affricate and plosive are devoiced first 100% Final stage ・High vowels in the three environments are devoiced nearly 100% ・High vowels of the other environments are devoiced gradually 100% plosive affricate fricative 0% plosive affricate fricative e.g. Osaka Japanese speakers in their 40s and older ・High vowels are devoiced almost 100 % except high vowels between fricative/affricate and fricative ・The overall average devoicing rate is over 80% 100% plosive affricate fricative 0% plosive affricate fricative e.g. Kumamoto Japanese speakers in their 40s and older Akita Japanese speakers in their 60s and older plosive affricate fricative 0% plosive affricate fricative e.g. Tokyo Japanese speakers in all age groups 21 devoicing rate % devoicing rate % • Devoicing rates in four areas are increasing roughly with age • With Akita and Kumamoto, younger generation’s devoicing rate has reached over 80 %, the final stage, which is the same stage as Tokyo • Hyogo and Osaka’s devoicing rates are under 60% in all age groups • As a result, a regional as well as a generational difference remains devoicing rate % • Five representative areas • Devoicing rate in Tokyo is over 80% in all age groups devoicing rate % Takada Data devoicing rate % RESULT 100 80 TOKYO 60 40 20 0 1900 1910 1920 1930 1940 1950 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 100 80 AKITA 60 40 20 0 1900 1910 1920 1930 1940 1950 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 100 80 60 40 20 KUMAMOTO 0 1900 1910 1920 1930 1940 1950 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 100 80 HYOGO 60 40 20 0 1900 1910 1920 1930 1940 1950 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 100 80 OSAKA 60 40 20 • Now, can we call this synchronic 0 1900 generational difference a diachronic change? 1910 1920 1930 1940 1950 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 22 Discussion • • • • Cohort analysis & apparent time Age, period and cohort effects Devoicing rate of same cohort The line of demarcation of devoicing rate • • • • Social factors of common Japanization Factors for change of devoicing rate S-curve model Future devoicing rate 23 DISCUSSION Cohort Analysis & Apparent Time • A cohort is a group of people who show a common characteristic within a certain period (e.g. a group of people who were born in 1970 and educated in the same area) • In the studies of change in progress, the notion of “apparent time” has generally been used for explaining a diachronic change from synchronic data • “Apparent time” assumes that the grammar (including phonetics and phonology) of individuals becomes relatively stable after the end of the language acquisition period and remains the way of speaking during their lifetime. Thus, speech differences among people of different ages reflect characteristics they acquired in their formative years (Cedergren 1987, Chambers 2002) • From this, we assume that same birth cohort shows great similarity in the use of language at any time of investigation ⇒cohort effect • There are two more effects: age effect, where people change as they get older, and period effect, where society changes affect people regardless of their age • Especially, the age effect (age-grading) must be differentiated from cohort effect when we deal with a diachronic change 24 DISCUSSION Age, Period and Cohort Effects (Glenn 1977) age effect only AGE % 60s 50s 40s 30s 20s 10s 1960 40 50 60 70 80 90 1970 40 50 60 70 80 90 1980 40 50 60 70 80 90 1990 40 50 60 70 80 90 2000 40 50 60 70 80 90 2010 40 50 60 70 80 90 … 60 70 80 … ・ The value of AGE (column) is changing …60 70 80… ・ The value of YEAR (row) does NOT change ・ The value of COHORT (skew) is changing 40… 60 70 80… YEAR of investigation YEAR of investigation % period effect only cohort effect only AGE % 60s 50s 40s 30s 20s 10s 1960 40 40 40 40 40 40 1970 50 50 50 50 50 50 1980 60 60 60 60 60 60 : 60 1990 70 70 70 70 70 70 70 2000 80 80 80 80 80 80 2010 90 90 90 90 90 90 80 : YEAR of investigation fictitious devoicing rate AGE 60s 50s 40s 30s 20s 10s 1960 20 30 40 50 60 70 1970 30 40 50 60 70 80 1980 40 50 60 70 80 90 1990 50 60 70 80 90 100 2000 60 70 80 90 100 100 2010 70 80 90 100 100 100 … 60 70 ・ The value of AGE does NOT change ・ The value of YEAR is changing …60 70 80… ・ The value of COHORT is changing 40… 60 70 80… 80 … ・ The values of AGE and YEAR are changing ・ The value of between COHORTs is changing… 60 70 80… ・The value of within COHORT does NOT change …60 60 60… Two of the three factors are related to each other 25 DISCUSSION Age, Period and Cohort Effects fictitious devoicing rate AGE effect AGE % 60s 50s 40s 30s 20s 10s COHORT effect AGE 60s 50s 40s 30s 20s 10s % AGE 60s 50s 40s 30s 20s 10s 50 60 70 80 90 1960 40 40 40 40 40 40 1960 20 30 40 50 60 70 1970 40 50 60 70 80 90 1970 50 50 50 50 50 50 1970 30 40 50 60 70 80 1980 40 50 60 70 80 90 1980 60 60 60 60 60 60 1980 40 50 60 70 80 90 1990 40 50 60 70 80 90 1990 70 70 70 70 70 70 1990 50 60 70 80 90 100 YEAR 1960 40 YEAR YEAR % PERIOD effect One point in time in an apparent-time study, for example, 1970 • AGE effect is likely to be judged as a diachronic change because the values are changing with age. On the other hand, we will not confuse PERIOD (year) effect with a diachronic change because of no change with age Two points in time, for example, 1970 and 1980 • There is no change between two times in AGE effect ⇒ age-grading • There is a change between two times in COHORT effect ⇒ diachronic change ● We can confirm whether a generational difference in synchronic data is age-grading or a diachronic change by comparing data with two or more different points in time 26 DISCUSSION Devoicing Rate of Same Cohort devoicing rate % • Unfortunately, due to insufficient data, it is impossible to compare the two data sets by each cohort ○:Inoue Data • It suggests that age difference in devoicing rate of each area is not agegrading, but a sound change in progress devoicing rate % • No statistically significant differences between 1970 cohorts of the two data sets in four areas devoicing rate % • 1970 cohorts in the two data sets show a striking similarity between average devoicing rates devoicing rate % • We focus on 1970 (1966-1975) cohorts 100 80 + :Takada Data AKITA 60 40 20 0 1900 1910 1920 1930 1940 1950 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 100 80 60 40 20 KUMAMOTO 0 1900 1910 1920 1930 1940 1950 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 100 80 HYOGO 60 40 20 0 1900 1910 1920 1930 1940 1950 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 100 80 OSAKA 60 40 20 0 1900 1910 1920 1930 1940 1950 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 DISCUSSION • Osaka is likely to have the line in 1970 • Hyogo might have the line in 1970 ( Evidence from results of word accents ) • There is no clear line of demarcation in Tokyo as noted in previous studies • The difference among areas can be explained in terms of standardization of dialects (common Japanization) devoicing rate % 100 80 TOKYO 60 40 No ○ marks in Tokyo 20 0 devoicing rate % 100 80 AKITA 60 40 20 0 1900 1910 1920 1930 1940 1950 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 devoicing rate % • Akita and Kumamoto have the line of demarcation approximately 1950 and 1970 respectively +:Takada Data 1900 1910 1920 1930 1940 1950 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 100 80 60 40 20 KUMAMOTO 0 1900 1910 1920 1930 1940 1950 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 devoicing rate % • Individual devoicing rate in five areas of Takada Data • Some areas’ devoicing rate increased dramatically, and others gradually 100 80 HYOGO 60 40 20 0 1900 1910 1920 1930 1940 1950 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 devoicing rate % The Line of Demarcation ○:Inoue Data 100 80 OSAKA 60 40 20 0 1900 1910 1920 1930 1940 1950 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 DISCUSSION Social Factors of Common Japanization • Inoue (1985) and Yoneda (1997) suggested some social factors of common Japanization from the study on the standardization of modern Japanese in Tsuruoka city, conducted by the National Language Research Institute in 1950, 1971, and 1991 • Three main factors are (1) education of standardization, (2) improvement of transport, (3) acquisition of Tokyo Japanese through TV programs • These social factors can also explain why the lines of demarcation of Akita and Kumamoto exhibit around 1950 and 1970. Akita is to do with (1), Kumamoto (2) (3) • As for Osaka and Hyogo, these factors seem to have been little effect 29 DISCUSSION Factors for Change of Devoicing Rate • • • • • Standard Japanese education of “spoken” language began in earnest in elementary school after 1941, and Akita (largely Tohoku) was among the areas where educators tried aggressively to correct thick accents. It seems that the result of education appeared from the 1950 birth cohort Kumamoto is likely to be affected by transport and TV. During the years of steep economic growth, transport had improved tremendously, increasing a wide range of interaction with others The Royal Wedding in 1959 and the Tokyo Olympic Games in 1964 carried a TV penetration rate to over 90 % in 1965. Yoneda (1997) says Tokyo Japanese through television must have given an impulse to the lower age groups, which are the most sensitive to language Acquisition of the Tokyo accent of younger generation led to an increase in devoicing rate Here are word accents produced by Kumamoto Japanese speakers and their devoicing rates: 100% Types of word accent (total 100 %) 12% 86% 0% 60’s 29% 100% 29% 91% 67% 71% 50’s 40’s 87% 92% LH HL HH 9% 30’s 13% 20’s 8% 10’s The vowel of the first syllable is the target vowel 0% -100% Devoicing rate by word accent (each 100%) 100% 60% 58% 6% 15% 3% 60’s 50’s 40’s 88% 87% 83% 25% 28% 20% 30’s 20’s 10’s LH HL 30 DISCUSSION S-curve Model Percent adoption (Devoicing rate) 100% 0% S-shaped curve of language change slow (Aitchison 2001) quick If the generational difference in devoicing rate is a sound change, the devoicing rate would increase in line with an S-curve quick slow Time 100 80 In many cases, but not all, a diffusion process follows an S-shaped curve 60 TV color TV car CD player 40 The penetration rate of durable goods based on Consumer0 Confidence Survey 2004 by Cabinet Office, Government of Japan 1957 1958 1959 1960 1961 1962 1963 1964 1965 1966 1967 1968 1969 1970 1971 1972 1973 1974 1975 1976 1977 1978 1979 1980 1981 1982 1983 1984 1985 1986 1987 1988 1989 1990 1991 1992 1993 1994 1995 1996 1997 1998 1999 2000 2001 2002 2003 2004 20 DISCUSSION Future Devoicing Rate • A vertical dotted line represents the final stage ①TOKYO 100 Devoicing rate % • The devoicing rate of the areas in the process of change is approaching the final stage on a wave of common Japanization • The younger age groups of Akita and Kumamoto have reached the final stage, and will maintain the status like Tokyo • Osaka and Hyogo are in the process of change, adopting devoiced vowels (an upward trend) 80 ③KUMAMOTO 60 40 ②AKITA ④HYOGO 20 ⑤OSAKA 30 years about 80 years 0 60s 30s 50s 20s 40s 10s 30s 30s 20s 20s 10s 10s 60s 50s 40s 60s 50s 40s 60s 50s 40s 30s 20s 10s ④⑤ ① 60s 60s 50s 40s 30s 20s 10s ② 60s 50s 40s 30s 20s 10s 50s 40s 30s 20s 10s ③ 60s 50s 40s 30s 20s 10s • If Osaka keeps changing rapidly like Akita or Kumamoto, younger speakers would reach the final stage in 30 years, and older speakers in about 80 years • Hyogo might take more time since it is more sluggish than Osaka 32 References • • • • • Aitchison, J. (2001) Language change: progress or decay? 3rd Edition. Fontana Press. Cedergren, H. J. (1987) The spread of language change: verifying inferences of linguistic diffusion. In P. H. Lowenberg (ed.), Language Spread and Language Policy: Issues, Implications and Case Studies. pp. 44-60. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Roundtable on Language and Linguistics. Chambers, J. K. (2002) Patterns of Variation including Change. In J. K. Chambers, P. Trudgill and N. Schilling-Estes (eds.), The Handbook of Language Variation and change. pp. 349-372. Oxford: Basil Blackwell. Glenn, N. D. (1977) Cohort analysis. SAGE Publications. Yoneda, M. (1997) Survey of standardization in Tsuruoka, Japan: comparison of results from three surveys conducted at 20-year intervals. Japanese linguistics 2, 24-39. • 井上史雄(1985 1985)『新しい日本語-《新方言》の分布と変化-』明治書院 (Inoue, 1985) • 井上史雄(1989)『日本語音声の地域差・世代差の音韻論的・音響学的分析』 昭和63(1988)年度科学研究費助成金 総合(A)研 究成果報告書 (Inoue, 1989) • NHK放送文化研究所編(1998)『NHK 日本語発音アクセント辞典 新版』日本放送出版協会 (Nihon Hoso Kyokai, 1998) • 金田一春彦(1954)「音韻」 『日本方言学』吉川弘文館, 87-176 (Kindaichi, 1954) • 佐藤和之(1985)「方言の音声と音韻」『新しい方言音声』(加藤正信編)至文堂, 29-46 (Sato, 1985) • • 沢木幹栄(2005)「周辺的要素の地域差」『日本方言研究会 第80回研究発表会 発表原稿集』89-92 (Sawaki, 2005) 高田三枝子(2009)『日本語語頭有声破裂音におけるVOTの地理的・世代的多様性』2005年度第1回博報「ことばと文化・教育」研 究助成研究成果報告書 (Takada, 2009) • 日本放送協会編(1966)『 日本語発音アクセント辞典』日本放送出版協会 (Nihon Hoso Kyokai, 1966) • 平山輝男編(1960)『全国アクセント辞典』東京堂 (Hirayama, 1960) • 邊姫京(2007)「狭母音の無声化の全国的地域差と世代差」『日本語の研究』 3-1, 33-48 (Byun, 2007) • 邊姫京(2012)『日本語狭母音の無声化―共通語普及の指標として―』東京大学大学院総合文化研究科, 学位論文 • 馬瀬良雄(1977)「東西両方言の対立」『岩波講座 日本語11 方言』岩波書店, 235-289 (Mase, 1977) 33 Thank you Phonology Forum 2014
© Copyright 2024 ExpyDoc